Statements and Declarations
The authors have no competing interests to declare that are relevant to the content of this article. The datasets analysed in this study are available to download from the UK Data Service (Wave 1: Anders et al., 2024a; Wave 2: Anders et al., 2024b). The study received full ethical approval from the UCL Institute of Education Research Ethics Committee (REC1660). It is registered with the UCL Data Protection Office (Z6364106/2022/06/30).
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by UK Research and Innovation via their COVID-19 response fund [grant number ES/W001756/1] and their Economic and Social Research Council [grant number ES/X00015X/1]. For Felicity.
1 Introduction
The COVID-19 pandemic and the disruption it caused had substantial short-term effects on young people’s lives around the world, with evidence of significant impacts on young people’s wellbeing and mental health (De France et al., 2022; Wolf & Schmitz, 2024). Young people in England, the focus of this paper, were no exception: extended periods in which in-person schooling was suspended (Anders, 2024) interrupted both pupils’ learning (Jakubowski et al., 2024) and their social lives (Kalenkoski & Pabilonia, 2024), with consequent rises in loneliness a clear symptom of this (Kung et al., 2023). This widespread disruption had widely documented short-term effects on young people’s wellbeing (e.g. Attwood & Jarrold, 2023; Banks & Xu, 2020; Neugebauer et al., 2023; Newlove-Delgado et al., 2021; Quintana-Domeque & Zeng, 2023), the magnitude of which was found to be linked with the intensity of lockdown restrictions (Owens et al., 2022), and the immediacy of which is reflected in the way that wellbeing increased and decreased as restrictions tightened and eased (Creswell et al., 2021). A review by Kauhanen et al. (2023) summarised the international picture as “a longitudinal deterioration in symptoms for different mental health outcomes especially for adolescents and young people”.
Existing analyses suggest that effects of the disruption were unequal, often exacerbating existing demographic inequalities in society. Previous studies from across the world have highlighted inequalities associated with socioeconomic status (e.g., Ravens-Sieberer et al., 2022), gender (e.g., Anders et al., 2023; Davillas & Jones, 2021), ethnicity (e.g., Proto & Quintana-Domeque, 2021), and intersections of these characteristics. There is also evidence that older adolescents were particularly affected (Wolf & Schmitz, 2024), perhaps as these are such formative years in terms of social relationships and critical years in terms of disruption to education affecting subsequent educational and school-to-work transitions.
A range of studies have also drawn attention to the importance of variation in experiences and support during the pandemic for young people’s wellbeing. Restrictions on social activities and the closure of schools reduced physical activity for some, which has been linked to worse mental health outcomes (Samji et al., 2022); other aspects of the pandemic and its restrictions are likely to have exacerbated the prevalence of adverse life events that previous studies have shown affect wellbeing (Cleland et al., 2016). On the other side of the ledger, the importance of social support has been identified has a potential buffer to negative impacts (Racine et al., 2021) of such negative stressors. These highlight the potential importance of experiences and social support during the pandemic for young people’s wellbeing and, hence, the need to consider these in understanding differences in wellbeing.
While short-term impacts are, of course, important in their own right, we should be especially concerned if the impacts of the pandemic are continuing to affect young people’s lives, including their subjective wellbeing, now that restrictions have subsided and life is back to ‘normal’. Concern was expressed from early in the pandemic that negative effects of the pandemic on wellbeing would persist beyond the end of restrictions (Sonuga-Barke & Fearon, 2021), something that emerging evidence from the general population suggests may be being borne out (Quintana-Domeque & Proto, 2022).
Informed by the findings of these studies, we seek to provide new evidence regarding ongoing differences in young people’s wellbeing post-pandemic, including those driven by their demographic characteristics, the role of adverse life experiences during the pandemic, and the potential buffering role of social support. In doing so, we are guided by sociological theoretical models of stress processes in shaping wellbeing (Pearlin, 1989), distinguishing between long-term stressors such as those linked with socioeconomic and demographic characteristics, and more acute ones such as those presented by the disruption of the pandemic and specific events during its course. We also seek to provide new evidence to illuminate the relevance of young people’s own perceptions of the ongoing impacts of the pandemic for their mental wellbeing through quantifying the extent to which young people’s self-reports of such impacts are associated with their post-pandemic subjective wellbeing.
In particular, our research aims are to: 1. estimate differences in post-pandemic wellbeing among this cohort by demographic characteristics; 2. validate and quantify young people’s own perceptions of the impact of the pandemic on their wellbeing and; 3. consider the role of adverse experiences during the pandemic — and how they may interact with existing predictors of wellbeing — in explaining differences in post-pandemic wellbeing.
To achieve these aims, we use data from the COVID Social Mobility & Opportunities study (COSMO) — a representative cohort study of over 13,000 young people in England aged 14-15 at pandemic onset whose education and post-16 transitions were acutely affected by the pandemic’s disruption through their remaining education and subsequent transitions — to explore young people’s subjective wellbeing since the end of most restrictions linked to the pandemic. COSMO has collected data on wellbeing at two annual, post-pandemic surveys (to date), along with rich data on demographics, social resources and experiences during the pandemic, allowing us to explore post-pandemic patterns in wellbeing and how they are shaped by these factors.
The paper proceeds as follows. In Section 2, we describe the data that we use in more detail, the steps we take to prepare it for analysis, and conduct descriptive analyses and visualisation to provide initial evidence on our research aims. In Section 3, we describe our use of regression modelling to support our analyses, before presenting results of this modelling in Section 4. Finally, in Section 5, we conclude with a discussion of our findings and their implications for policy and practice.
2 Data and descriptive analyses
This study uses data from the COVID Social Mobility & Opportunities study (COSMO). This longitudinal cohort study recruited a representative sample of young people (and their parents) who were in Year 10 (i.e., aged 14-15) at the onset of the pandemic in March 2020. We use data from those who participated at both waves 1 (Anders et al., 2024a), which was carried out between October 2021 and March 2022 (while participants were aged 16-17), and 2 (Anders et al., 2024b), which was carried out between October 2022 and March 2023 (while participants were aged 17-18). In both cases the majority of interviews are carried out within the first two months of fieldwork; we also control for month of interview in our regression models (further details below).
COSMO has a clustered and stratified design with oversampling of those from smaller (e.g., ethnic minorities), more disadvantaged and harder to reach demographic groups, aiming to allow for larger than proportional samples of those smaller groups to improve statistical power when exploring inequalities between such groups. Furthermore, there was initial non-response and attrition between the two waves. As such, it is important to take into account the deliberate and modelled disproportionalities in our sample, as well as the implications of the clustering and stratification for statistical inference; we seek to take these features into account in all our analyses using R’s survey package (Lumley et al., 2024) with the study’s provided clustering and stratification variables, and design and non-response weights (Adali et al., 2022, 2023).
To ensure a consistent sample across analyses, we restrict the sample to those who have valid data on the key variables we will be using in our analyses. This includes the primary outcome variable of self-reported wellbeing score, as well as the key predictors and demographic variables of which we make use. However, we are mindful of the potential implications of sample selection caused by complete case analysis, so we robustness check our results to ensure that our findings are not driven by the exclusion of those with missing data in Section 7, re-running our core analyses having only restricted the sample based on the primary outcome (wellbeing score) and the main predictors (impact of pandemic on mental health and adverse life events reporting) and multiply imputed across 10 datasets all other predictors using a highly flexible classification and regression tree approach (Lumley, 2019; van Buuren & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, 2023).
2.1 Subjective wellbeing
To measure self-reported wellbeing, we use the UK Office for National Statistics’ official measure of life satisfaction (Office for National Statistics, 2018), which is widely recognised as an important dimension of subjective wellbeing (Petersen et al., 2022). This asks participants to respond to the prompt “Overall, how satisfied are you with your life nowadays?” on a scale ranging from 0 “Not at all satisfied” to 10 “Completely satisfied”. This measure has been used in national UK surveys since 2011 and increasing numbers of academic studies, hence providing a useful benchmark for this concept in surveys based in the UK. There is also evidence that this measure is reliable measure of subjective wellbeing in young people (Levin & Currie, 2014), performing as well as the more in-depth Satisfaction with Life Scale (Jovanović, 2016), for example, although we do recognise that it will not capture all dimension of wellbeing (Ruggeri et al., 2020). It is also worth noting that, while they are distinct constructs, a clear correlation between lower wellbeing and increased risk of poor mental health (Lombardo et al., 2018).
As COSMO was established in response to the pandemic, there are no baseline pre-pandemic measures. As such, we emphasise that our estimates of differences in subjective wellbeing are between individuals all of whom have experienced the pandemic, but may have experienced it in different ways, rather than between their current situation and a counterfactual in which the pandemic did not happen. Others have used pre-existing longitudinal studies to explore change in mental health across the pandemic period (Henseke, mimeo), or attempted to get closer to such a counterfactual using a survey experiment approach explicitly asking participants to imagine the scenario where the pandemic had not happened (Andreoli et al., 2024).
We have measures of wellbeing at two waves post-pandemic and use these to explore evidence of change in wellbeing between the two waves both overall, and between sub-groups of the data where this might be expected. We plot the overall distribution of reported wellbeing in both Waves 1 (age 16/17) and 2 (age 17/18) in Figure 1.
Young people report a mean wellbeing score of 6.41 in Wave 1 and 6.43 at Wave 2, with the spread of the measure declining slightly from 2.05 to 1.97, but these are not a particularly substantial change. This provides little evidence of change between these two post-pandemic time points. However, in attempting to interpreting this (lack of) aggregate change, we must be mindful of wider context for this cohort.
One interpretation would be that, as we know there was a decline in mental health and wellbeing among young people at the onset of the pandemic and its restrictions (Newlove-Delgado et al., 2021), we would hope to see an upward trajectory in wellbeing in subsequent years to be confident of a ‘bounce back’, with this lack of change suggesting a plateau at a lower level than was the case before the pandemic. That could be the case. A finding of minimal change is consistent with the findings of Henseke et al. (2022) (albeit for a wider age range of young people aged 16-29). Similarly, the UK Office For National Statistics’ annual population survey also suggests that life satisfaction has not returned to pre-pandemic levels in the general population (Office for National Statistics, 2023).
Fundamentally, using our data alone we are unable to adjudicate between multiple potential plausible scenarios. Others, using a wider range of datasets are better placed to do so. For example, Henseke (mimeo) suggest that young people’s wellbeing may have already returned to pre-pandemic levels, thus explaining a lack of trend for this reason. These findings would also be consistent with an upward post-pandemic trend being cancelled out by a countervailing age effect (for example) that would be expected based on the wider literature on wellbeing across the life course (Blanchflower, 2021).
However, this is not the focus of our paper. This aggregate stability at the cohort level does not mean that there are not individual-level differences or differential change in reported wellbeing. The correlation between the reported measures in Waves 1 and 2 is 0.54. While some of this likely reflects the natural fluctuation in young people’s wellbeing due to idiosyncratic shocks that hit their lives every day, we now go on to explore evidence of systematic difference in change between the two waves, along with the differences in levels at each wave.
2.3 Perceived ongoing impact
Next, we seek to quantify differences in young people’s wellbeing by their own perceptions of the ongoing impact of the pandemic. This takes seriously young people’s own perceptions of the ongoing impact of the pandemic on their wellbeing. To capture these perceptions, we use a question asked to young people at the second wave of COSMO. Specifically, young people are asked “Would you say the pandemic is still having an effect on [your mental wellbeing], whether positive or negative?” If they agree with this question then they are subsequently asked to distinguish whether this impact is positive, negative or they don’t know.
Variable, N = 7723 | No (64%)1 | Negative (32%)1 | Don’t know (2%)1 | Positive (2%)1 | Overall (100%)1 | p-value2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Wave 1 | 6.81 | 5.62 | 6.37 | 6.40 | 6.41 | <0.001 |
Wave 2 | 6.91 | 5.46 | 6.29 | 6.48 | 6.43 | <0.001 |
Difference | 0.11 | -0.16 | -0.08 | 0.08 | 0.02 | <0.001 |
Notes: All estimates are weighted and account for the complex survey design. The difference is calculated as Wave 2 - Wave 1. | ||||||
1 Mean | ||||||
2 Design-based KruskalWallis test |
Table 3 shows that 64% of young people report that the pandemic is continuing to have an impact on their mental wellbeing, with 32% of these reporting that this impact is negative. Perhaps unsurprisingly, much smaller proportion of young people report that the ongoing impact is positive (2%) or that they don’t know if the impact is positive or negative (2%).
Those who report no impact of the pandemic on their mental wellbeing have the highest self-reported wellbeing (6.37 in Wave 1; 6.29 in Wave 2), while those who report that it had a negative impact on their mental wellbeing report the lowest (5.62 in Wave 1; 5.46 in Wave 2). Those who say it is still having an impact but that it is positive, or that they don’t know if it is positive or negative, report somewhere between the other two groups but, as noted above, these are a very small proportion of the sample.
These groups are also distinguished by the change in their reported wellbeing between Waves 1 and 2. Those who report that the pandemic is continuing to have a negative impact on their mental wellbeing do, indeed, report a decline in wellbeing (-0.16) between the two waves, while those who report that it has had no impact (-0.08) or that it is having a positive impact report an increase (0.08). Those who report that it is still having an impact but that they don’t know if it is positive or negative report a slight decline (0.11).
These last two groups are small, so these estimates should be treated with caution, and in subsequent analyses we decide to combine these two groups with the group who report no impact. This allows for an overall comparison of those who report an ongoing negative impact with the rest of the sample in our later regression analyses seeking to understand the interplay of these perceptions further.
2.4 Adverse life events
Finally, we are interested in understanding whether subjective wellbeing is affected by adverse life events that happened during the COVID pandemic. In Wave 1, COSMO asked participants whether they had experienced each of the following life events since the onset of the pandemic in March 2020:
- A parent/guardian or carer lost their job or business
- My family could not afford to buy enough food, or had to use a food bank
- My family could not afford to pay their bills/rent/mortgage
- I was seriously ill in hospital
- A close family member or friend is or was seriously ill in hospital
- A close family member or friend died
- Increase in number of arguments with parents/guardians
- Increase in number of arguments between parents/guardians
- Moving to a new home
- Parents/guardians separated or divorced
The question is worded to capture events whether or not they are directly attributable to the pandemic, its restrictions and disruptions, but it is reasonable to believe many were caused or exacerbated by the circumstances of the pandemic. Participants were then asked again whether they had experienced these events over the past twelve months (i.e., for most participants a year since they responded to the Wave 1 survey) in Wave 2.
29% of pupils experience no events at all, while 26% experience three or more events. We report the proportion of young people experiencing each of the ten specific adverse life events in the first column of Table 4.
We initially anticipated using the simple count of number of adverse events experienced in analyses. However, the substantial differences in prevalence of the events means this would be inappropriately imposing the same importance, or severity, of all the events. Instead, we want to allow these to differ, such that lower probability/higher impact events are given more weight in our analysis. We therefore create a composite index of adverse life events using a polychoric principal component analysis (PCA) of the ten measured adverse life events.
The first principal component explains 32% of the variance in the ten adverse life events. We standardise this index to have mean 0 and standard deviation 1 in our analysis sample, plot the distribution in Figure 4, and use it to split it into three groups based on the tertiles of the index (accounting for sample weighting). We label these groups as “Low”, “Medium” and “High” to reflect the relative impact of adverse events experienced by each of these groups.
Variable, N = 7723 | Low (36%)1 | Medium (30%)1 | High (33%)1 | Overall (100%)1 |
---|---|---|---|---|
Parent lost job | 0 | 13 | 23 | 12 |
Couldn't afford food | 0 | 2.8 | 23 | 8.4 |
Couldn't afford bills | 0 | 4.6 | 28 | 11 |
Seriously ill | 0 | 2.6 | 7.0 | 3.1 |
Close family member seriously ill | 0 | 45 | 54 | 32 |
Close family member died | 19 | 28 | 51 | 33 |
More arguments with parents | 0 | 28 | 72 | 32 |
More arguments between parents | 0 | 7.8 | 60 | 22 |
Moved home | 0 | 6.6 | 16 | 7.3 |
Parents separated | 0 | 1.5 | 10 | 3.8 |
Number of events (grouped) | ||||
0 | 81 | 0 | 0 | 29 |
1 | 19 | 60 | 0 | 25 |
2 | 0 | 40 | 23 | 20 |
3+ | 0 | 0 | 77 | 26 |
Number of events (mean) | 0.19 | 1.40 | 3.44 | 1.64 |
Notes: All estimates are weighted and account for the complex survey design. | ||||
1 %; Mean |
We report the prevalence of each of the ten adverse life events by the three groups of the index in Table 4. This demonstrates that these groups are capturing different levels of exposure to adverse life events, while reflecting the differential prevalence of the ten events. Students in the “Low” group are likely not to have experienced any of the events, with the exception of a close family member dying. In contrast, students in the “High” group have a good chance of having experienced multiple events.
Variable, N = 7723 | Low (36%)1 | Medium (30%)1 | High (33%)1 | Overall (100%)1 | p-value2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Wave 1 | 7.04 | 6.46 | 5.67 | 6.41 | <0.001 |
Wave 2 | 7.02 | 6.47 | 5.75 | 6.43 | <0.001 |
Difference | -0.03 | 0.00 | 0.08 | 0.02 | 0.2 |
Notes: All estimates are weighted and account for the complex survey design. | |||||
1 Mean | |||||
2 Design-based KruskalWallis test |
We find that mean wellbeing score differs by experience of such events (Table 5). Wellbeing is lower for those who experience a higher prevalence of adverse life events, ranging from 5.67 for those with low experience of adverse life events to 6.46 for those with a high level of experience of these. This pattern is consistent across Waves 1 and 2, but there is no significant evidence of difference in the patterns of change over time.
However, as with all our descriptive analyses, we are mindful that there is the potential for a lot of differences in socioeconomic and demographic characteristics between those who experience adverse life events and those who do not. For this reason, as well as for our other analyses, we use regression models to help us unpack these findings further.
3 Regression analysis
To extend our descriptive analyses and, hence, provide a more nuanced understanding of the factors associated with young people’s wellbeing since the pandemic, we use regression models. All analyses are carried out using the statistical software R (R Core Team, 2024), with the survey package (Lumley et al., 2024) used to account for the complex survey design of the data, including design and non-response weights, along with adjustments to statistical inference due to stratification and clustering of the sample.
We break this section into three sub-sections, aligned with the research aims in this paper for which we will use regression modelling to support our analysis: demographic differences in subjective wellbeing; the importance of perceived ongoing impact of the pandemic; and the importance of adverse life events during the pandemic.
3.1 Demographic differences in subjective wellbeing
First, we use linear regression models to explore differences in young people’s wellbeing. These models all take the form
\(LifeSat_{it} = \beta_0 + \beta'_{1} SES_{i} + \beta'_{2} Gender_{i} + \beta'_{3} Ethnicity_{i} + X'_{i} + \varepsilon_{it}\)
where \(LifeSat\) is the wellbeing score for person \(i\) at time \(t\), \(SES\) is a vector of binary variables for the quintile groups of SES (leaving the highest SES quintile group as the omitted category), \(Gender\) is a vector of binary variables for gender variables (Female and Non-binary+, leaving Male as the omitted category), \(Ethnicity\) is a vector of binary variables for ethnicity (White, Asian, Black, Mixed, Other, leaving the omitted category as Mixed), \(X\) is a vector of other covariates, which varies between model specifications (discussed further below), and \(\varepsilon\) is the error term. We estimate these models separately for each time point of the survey, and then again for Wave 2 with an additional covariate of Wave 1 wellbeing score to provide estimates of difference adjusting for Wave 1 wellbeing as a baseline.
We begin with simple models including gender (L1), ethnicity (L2), and SES (L3) entered separately, replicating the descriptive analyses and unconditional estimates of differences in wellbeing reported in Table 2. Next, we include all three demographic characteristics at the same time in L4, along with the addition of a month of interview variable to allow for potential confounding due to the timing of the survey. This model, hence, provides estimates of demographic differences in wellbeing, conditional on the other demographic characteristics included.
We then explore evidence of intersectional differences in wellbeing between our core demographics in L5 (Codiroli Mcmaster & Cook, 2019) by including a full set of interaction terms between our SES, gender and ethnicity variables.
Next, motivated by understanding the potential importance of social resources in explaining these differences, we add social provisions score in L6. Differences between the coefficients on our demographic characteristics between L4 and L6 will, hence, provide information on the extent to which differences in these resources may explain the unadjusted differences.
L7 explores whether the importance of social resources varies by demographic characteristics. As with L5, we include interaction terms, this time between our demographic characteristics and the two social resources measures to allow for the moderation of the relationship between these measures and wellbeing.
Finally, L8 explores the importance of adverse life events in explaining demographic differences in wellbeing. We include the adverse life events index in this model, along with the demographic characteristics and social resources measures. Comparing coefficients on the demographic characteristics in L6 and L8 will, hence, provide information on the extent to which differences in adverse life events may explain demographic differences in wellbeing. We do not run a model exploring the interaction between adverse life events and demographic characteristics at this point as we will explore this in a subsequent section.
3.2 Importance of perceived impact of the pandemic on wellbeing
In this section, we again use linear regression models to estimate differences in subjective wellbeing. However, this time we focus on differences explained by young people’s perceptions of the ongoing impact of the pandemic on their life, allowing us to validate and quantify these reports. The models used for this purpose all take the form:
\(LifeSat_{it} = \beta_0 + \beta'_{1} PandemicImpactPercep_{i} + X'_{i} + \varepsilon_{it}\)
where \(LifeSat\) is the wellbeing score for person \(i\) at time \(t\), \(PandemicImpactPercep\) is a binary variable indicating that person \(i\) reports that the pandemic is continuing to have a negative impact on their life, \(X\) is a vector of other covariates, which varies between model specifications (discussed further below), and \(\varepsilon\) is the error term. We estimate these models separately for each time point of the survey, and then again for Wave 2 with an additional covariate of Wave 1 wellbeing to provide estimates of difference adjusting for Wave 1 wellbeing as a baseline.
Our first model (P1) again replicates our descriptive findings by including no additional covariates, meaning the coefficient on \(PandemicImpactPercep\) reports the difference between those who report that the pandemic had a negative impact on their mental wellbeing and the rest of the cohort (no longer disaggregating the ‘don’t know’ and ‘positive’ groups).
Next, in P2, we include demographic (gender, ethnicity) and socioeconomic status (parental education, housing tenure, and area-level deprivation) covariates. We do this, rather than including combined SES quintile groups, now that we are not trying to interpret an overall SES association but rather adjust for these as flexibly as possible. We also include month of survey at this point. Our focal coefficient from this model thus estimates the difference in wellbeing associated with a continuing negative perception of the pandemic on wellbeing among those with similar socio-demographic characteristics.
We then explore the extent to which differences in wellbeing associated with a negative perceived impact of the pandemic are explained by the social resources available to young people. In P3, we add social provisions score and compare the estimate on our focal variable coefficient between models P2 and P3.
Finally, in P4, we explore evidence of variation in the difference in wellbeing associated with a negative perceived impact of the pandemic by demographic and social support measures. We do this by including a full set of interaction terms between our focal variable and the socio-demographic and social support variables in P3. Examining the coefficients on these interaction terms will provide evidence on this point.
3.3 Importance of adverse life events during the pandemic
For the paper’s final research aim, we explore the importance of adverse life events during the pandemic in explaining young people’s wellbeing post-pandemic.
To do so, we use linear regression models to explore the extent to which differences in self-reported wellbeing depends on the adverse life experiences they faced, including conditional on their perception of the impact of the pandemic on their wellbeing. The models used for this purpose all take the form:
\(LifeSat_{it} = \beta_0 + \beta'_{1} TAdverseEventIndex_{i} + X'_{i} + \varepsilon_{it}\)
where \(LifeSat\) is the wellbeing score for person \(i\) at time \(t\), \(TAdverseEventIndex\) is a vector of binary variables indicating person \(i\)’s location in the distribution of the adverse life event index (high and medium, leaving low as a baseline), \(X\) is a vector of other covariates, which varies between model specifications (discussed further below), and \(\varepsilon\) is the error term. We estimate these models separately for each time point of the survey, and then again for Wave 2 with an additional covariate of Wave 1 wellbeing to provide estimates of difference adjusting for Wave 1 wellbeing as a baseline. Where we are modelling wellbeing measured at Wave 1, a variant of our events index is used that is based on Wave 1 event reports only.
Our first model (E1) again replicates our descriptive findings by including only the tercile groups of the adverse life events index, meaning the coefficients on each level of \(TAdverseEventIndex\) report the difference between those who experience medium and high levels of adverse events, as applicable, compared to the low adverse life events group. In preliminary work to inform our approach, we explored alternative ways of including information on adverse life events in our modelling, including using the index as a continuous variable and including a set of binary variables for the individual adverse life events, as listed in Section 2. We found that including tercile groups provided the most interpretable results without substantively affecting the model fit.
Next, in E2, we add in demographic characteristics (gender, ethnicity) and socioeconomic status indicators (parental education, housing tenure, and area-level deprivation). We also include month of survey at this point. Our coefficient estimates associated with adverse life events from this model thus provides an estimate of difference in wellbeing associated with greater experiences of adverse life events during the pandemic among those with similar socio-demographic characteristics, as well as the extent to which differential distribution of such life events across socio-demographic groups explains differences in reported wellbeing.
We then explore the extent to which differences in wellbeing associated with greater experience of adverse life events during the pandemic are explained by the social resources available to young people. In E3, we add covariates for our social provisions scale scores and compare the estimate on our focal variable between models E2 and E3. It may be noted that this is very similar to model L6 from the earlier section but here adverse life events are our focus and, hence, are entered using the tercile groups to aide interpretation.
Next, we include the covariate for perceived ongoing impact of the pandemic that was the focal variable of the previous section. As we hypothesise that at least some of the formation of ongoing perceptions of negative impact from the pandemic, this model (E4) is likely not a reliable guide to the association between adverse events and wellbeing since including the perception variable is over-controlling. However, the model is useful as a point of comparison with P3 in demonstrating the extent to which the difference in wellbeing associated with a negative perception of the ongoing impact of the pandemic on wellbeing is explained by having experienced adverse life events.
Finally, analogously to previous sections, we include interactions of our focal variables (experience of adverse life events) with our socio-demographic and social support measures in model E5. This allows us to see if there is evidence of variation in the importance of having experienced adverse life events for post-pandemic wellbeing between different groups of young people.
4 Results
In this section, we discuss the results of the regression models outlined in the previous section. We begin by exploring demographic differences in wellbeing Section 4.1, before moving on to the importance of perceived ongoing impact of the pandemic Section 4.2 and the importance of adverse life events during the pandemic Section 4.3. We primarily report our results graphically (Larmarange, 2024), focussing attention on the estimates pertinent to addressing our research aims and allowing for easy comparison across models, supplemented with illustration of interactions between characteristics (Arel-Bundock et al., Forthcoming), where relevant. We also provide full regression tables of the results for each model, which are included in Section 6 for reference.
4.1 Demographic differences in subjective wellbeing
First, we explore overall differences in wellbeing, through a series of models summarised (for ease of reference) in Table 6. The core results are plotted in Figure 5 for gender, Figure 6 for ethnicity, and Figure 7 for SES. In each case, results are presented for Wave 1, Wave 2, and Wave 2 adjusted for Wave 1, with the discussion starting out with Wave 1 in each case, before focussing on any notable differences in the pattern for Wave 2, or Wave 2 adjusted for Wave 1. Full tables of results for these models are reported in Section 6, specifically Table 9 for Wave 1, Table 10 for Wave 2, and Table 11 for Wave 2 adjusted for Wave 1.
Variable | L1 | L2 | L3 | L4 | L5 | L6 | L7 | L8 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Gender | Included | Included | Interacted w/ Ethnicity and SES | Included | Interacted w/ Social Support | Included | ||
Ethnicity | Included | Included | Interacted w/ Gender and SES | Included | Interacted w/ Social Support | Included | ||
SES | Included | Included | Interacted w/ Gender and Ethnicity | Included | Interacted w/ Social Support | Included | ||
Social Support | Included | Interacted w/ Gender, Ethnicity and SES | Included | |||||
Adverse Events | Included |
Notes: L1-L7 refer to the model number. SES = Socioeconomic status.
Beginning with gender (Figure 5), we replicate the descriptive findings (Table 2) in model L1 (except for the inclusion of controls for month of interview), finding that girls’ wellbeing is 0.63 points lower than for boys, and a larger reduction for those grouped as non-binary+ where the reduction is 1.9 points compared to boys. There is essentially no change when we adjust for ethnicity and SES in model L4, with the differences remaining 0.63 points for girls and 1.9 points for non-binary+ young people.
However, some of the difference in levels of wellbeing for non-binary+ young people appears to be explained by variation in social support. When we include the social provisions scale in model L6, the difference in wellbeing reduces to 1.4 points for those identifying as non-binary+ compared to boys. This makes a similar difference for non-binary+ young people’s wellbeing at Wave 2, but no difference for girls at any wave, nor for non-binary+ youth when considering Wave 2 wellbeing after adjusting for their wellbeing at Wave 1.
A small part of the remaining difference in wellbeing appears to be explained by experiences of adverse life events, reducing to 1.2 for non-binary+ young people and to 0.5 for girls, although the difference between models L6 and L8 is not statistically significant for the non-binary+ group, nor quite statistically significant at the 5% level for girls.
Across Waves 1 and 2 (panels 1 and 2 of Figure 6), the only significant unconditional differences in young people’s wellbeing are between those classified as White and those classified as of Mixed ethnicity. No difference emerges when other demographic characteristics are included in model L4. However, these lower levels among those with Mixed ethnicity are explained by differences in social support, while, conversely, including this covariate reveals a significant difference in wellbeing between those classified as White and those classified as Black and Asian in model L6. This latter finding implies that if Black and Asian young people reported the same scores on the social provisions scale as White young people, their wellbeing scores would be higher. This difference is only present at Wave 2 for those with an Asian ethnicity, and is not present for any group when we are looking at Wave 2 wellbeing having adjusting for wellbeing at Wave 1.
The differences that emerged for Black and Asian young people in model L6 appear slightly attenuated by differences in adverse life events (0.16 for Black young people and 0.18 for Asian young people), but not by much and the estimates in L6 and L8 are not statistically significant from one another.
There is evidence of a gradient in wellbeing by SES, with a roughly linear pattern across SES quintile groups at both Waves 1 and 2. However, the differences are only significant in the unconditional model (L3) once we reach the top two quintile groups, compared to the bottom. The overall difference between the top and bottom quintile groups is 0.3 points at Wave 1 and a bit larger (0.33 points) at Wave 2.
There is essentially no difference when gender and ethnicity are included in model L4, but some of the SES gradient is attenuated by differences in social support when these are included in model L6. For Wave 1, the difference between the bottom and the second-highest quintile groups becomes statistically insignificant, although this is not the case for differences at Wave 2, given their slightly larger overall magnitude. The conditional difference between the top and bottom quintile groups is 0.21 points at Wave 1 and, again, a bit larger (0.24 points) at Wave 2.
Ultimately, even these differences between the top and bottom SES quintile groups are attenuated to statistical insignificance when we adjust for experiences of adverse life events in model L8 (although we should note that the differences in coefficients between models L6 and L8 are not themselves statistically significant). This is the case for both Waves 1 and 2, and for Wave 2 after adjusting for Wave 1 wellbeing. It would seem that, between them, we can account for much of the socioeconomic variation in wellbeing with social support and experiences of adverse life events — although it is important to note that this is not the same as saying that socioeconomic inequalities in wellbeing are unimportant, especially as socioeconomic status is likely to affect levels of social support and adverse life events.
We explore the potential for intersectional differences between the demographic characteristics using model L5, but find little evidence of any clear or consistent patterns of this type. Similarly, we allow for moderation of the importance of social support by demographic characteristics in model L7, but find little evidence of this either. Full tables of results for these models are reported the final two columns of Table 9 (Wave 1), Table 10 (Wave 2) and Table 11 (Wave 2 adjusting for Wave 1) in Section 6.
4.2 Perceived continuing impact of the pandemic on wellbeing
Next, we discuss differences in wellbeing by perceived continuing impact of the pandemic. Again, we summarise the models used to explore this issue in Table 7. The core results are plotted in Figure 8. Full tables of results for these models are reported in Section 6, Table 12 (Wave 1), Table 13 (Wave 2) and Table 14 (Wave 2 adjusting for Wave 1).
Variable | P1 | P2 | P3 | P4 |
---|---|---|---|---|
Perceived Impact | Included | Included | Included | Interacted with Demographics, SES and Social Support |
Demographics | Included | Included | Interacted with Perceived Impact | |
SES | Included | Included | Interacted with Perceived Impact | |
Social Support | Included | Interacted with Perceived Impact |
Notes: P1-P4 refer to the model number. SES = Socioeconomic status.
Results from unconditional model P1 indicate that young people who perceive a negative continuing impact of the pandemic on their wellbeing report 1.1 points lower wellbeing score than those who do not perceive such an impact. Perhaps surprisingly, given the greater time that has elapsed since the pandemic, this difference is larger at Wave 2, with a 1.4 point difference between these two groups. However, we should recall that the report of a negative continuing impact of the pandemic is collected at Wave 2, so may reflect this being more contemporary with the report.
A fairly small part of the difference in wellbeing score is explained by inclusion of demographic characteristics (in P2) and social support (in P3). The differences are reduced to 0.85 points and 1.2 points at Wave 1 and Wave 2, respectively, once all of these covariates have been included. This highlights a significant unexplained component of wellbeing unexplained by young people’s observable characteristics and social support — although we will return to whether more of this difference can be explained by adverse life events during the pandemic in the next section.
The unconditional difference in wellbeing by perceived continuing impact of the pandemic on wellbeing at Wave 2 is lower in models where we have adjusted for Wave 1 wellbeing (0.85 points). However, demographic and social support controls make essentially no difference for this outcome, with the difference remaining 0.81 points once these have been included, with a very similar magnitude to that seen in the fully adjusted model for Wave 1.
We also explore whether the difference associated with this perception is moderated by key demographic characteristics, particularly focussing on gender and social provisions. The results for gender are plotted in Figure 9 and suggest that the differences in wellbeing by perceived ongoing impact of the pandemic on wellbeing are larger for boys, although this is only statistically significant at the 10% level and for Wave 1 so this is rather a tentative finding. The results for social provisions are plotted in Figure 10 and suggest little variation in the difference in wellbeing by perceived ongoing impact of the pandemic on wellbeing depending on the level of social provisions.
4.3 Adverse life events
Next, we turn to the importance of adverse life events for young people’s wellbeing. This is explored through a series of models that, for ease of reference, are summarised in Table 8; full results are reported in Table 15, Table 16 and Table 17 in Section 6. The core results are plotted in Figure 11, demonstrating the association unconditionally (E1), adjusting for demographic measures (E2), and adjusting also for social support (E3).
Variable | E1 | E2 | E3 | E4 | E5 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Adverse Events | Included | Included | Included | Included | Interacted with Demographics, SES, Social Support and Perceived Impact |
Demographics | Included | Included | Included | Interacted with Adverse Events | |
SES | Included | Included | Included | Interacted with Adverse Events | |
Social Support | Included | Included | Interacted with Adverse Events | ||
Perceived Impact | Included | Interacted with Adverse Events |
Notes: E1-E5 refer to the model number. SES = Socioeconomic status.
Those who have experienced more adverse life events during the pandemic do report substantially lower wellbeing scores, with the unconditional difference between the low and high prevalence groups being 1.4 points at Wave 1 and 1.3 points at Wave 2. A small part of this difference is explained by demographic characteristics (in E2). However, more is explained by social support (in E3), especially in terms of the those who experienced the most adverse life events (i.e., the High tercile group), bringing the gap between low and high prevalence groups to 0.85 points at Wave 1 and 0.88 points at Wave 2.
As has become familiar, the patterns are similar but substantially attenuated when considering differences at Wave 2 that control for differences at Wave 1. Nevertheless, there remains a substantial difference (0.36 points) in wellbeing at Wave 2 by adverse events experienced even after controlling for wellbeing at Wave 1, demographic characteristics and social support.
Building on the models reported in Figure 11, we also explore whether the association between adverse life events and wellbeing is mediated by the perceived ongoing impact of the pandemic on wellbeing. The core results are plotted in Figure 12, which compares our model including the perceived ongoing impact of the pandemic on wellbeing (E4) with the analogous model excluding our events measure from earlier analyses (P3). The results suggest that, despite the differences in perceptions explained by adverse events experienced as discussed at the start of this section, only a fairly small part of the perceived ongoing impact of the pandemic on wellbeing is explained by the experience of adverse life events during the pandemic.
We did also explore whether there was evidence that adverse events matter more for some groups than others using interactions between adverse life events and key demographic characteristics. These results are reported in column E5 of Table 15, Table 16 and Table 17 in Section 6. There is little evidence of any systematic moderation of the main effects that we have discussed.
5 Conclusions
This paper contributes to existing literature on young people’s wellbeing in England in the aftermath of the COVID-19 pandemic by exploring levels of wellbeing at two time points since the pandemic and the factors associated with these levels. In particular, we build on existing work showing that the pandemic has had a negative impact on young people’s wellbeing (e.g., Mansfield et al., 2022), along with evidence of some initial recovery in wellbeing in the latter phases of the pandemic (Henseke et al., 2022).
We contribute to evidence on ongoing issues of gender differences in wellbeing, which other evidence demonstrates to have been exacerbated by the pandemic (e.g., Davillas & Jones, 2021), with girls and those who identify as non-binary or in another way reporting lower wellbeing scores (on a scale from 1-10 around 0.5 for girls; around 1.5 for non-binary+ young people) than boys, even after adjusting for other demographic characteristics, self-reported levels of social support and experience of adverse life events. These are substantial differences that are relevant to the higher rates of mental health challenges for those in these groups. In particular, the large differences associated with identifying as non-binary or in another way suggest the need for targeted support for those in this group.
Our analysis makes innovative use of young people’s own perceptions of the ongoing impact of the pandemic on their mental wellbeing in order to validate and quantify these reports. The findings of these analyses illustrate the importance of taking such reports seriously: those who indicate an ongoing negative impact in their lives have substantially lower subjective wellbeing scores — more than 1 point on a 1-10 scale — with similar differences across demographic groups.
Adverse life events experienced during the pandemic are shown to predict lower subjective wellbeing, although they can explain only a fairly small part of the lower scores we see among those who perceive an ongoing impact of the pandemic on their mental wellbeing. Contrary to our expectations, and in contrast to others’ findings (Racine et al., 2021), while social support predicts higher wellbeing scores, we did not find evidence that it mediates or buffers the impact of adverse life events in our population.
This study benefits from a large, representative, longitudinal dataset, with direct reports from both young people and parents to improve the quality of data collected. Nevertheless, in drawing these conclusions, we are mindful of the limitations of this study, most particularly that our data lacks any pre-pandemic baseline measures of wellbeing, which would substantially increase our ability to understand the longer-term dynamics of the changes (or lack thereof) in wellbeing that we have observed. We should also be aware that our data is drawn from a single cohort of young people in England, whose final years in compulsory education were especially disrupted by the impacts of the pandemic, which is important context in any attempt to generalise our findings to other populations.
Our findings indicate continuing challenges of inequalities in young people’s wellbeing and, hence, the importance of ongoing targeted support to overcome these. The practicalities of providing this at scale are now much harder for our specific cohort, since many of whom have now left education entirely, but many of the issues discussed will apply similarly to those still working their way through the education system who could be reached through schools and colleges. Ignoring this issue has potential implications for national economic performance (Deaton, 2008), including via the increased risk of mental health challenges implied (Lombardo et al., 2018), if more instrumental motivation is needed than simply the negative implications for the life experiences of these young people.
References
marginaleffects
in R
and Python
. Journal of Statistical Software. https://marginaleffects.com
6 Appendix: Full regression tables
Characteristic |
L1
|
L2
|
L3
|
L4
|
L5
|
L6
|
L7
|
L8
|
||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | |
(Intercept) | 6.7*** | 0.062 | 6.4*** | 0.057 | 6.2*** | 0.076 | 6.6*** | 0.087 | 6.5*** | 0.117 | 6.6*** | 0.081 | 6.6*** | 0.081 | 6.6*** | 0.079 |
Gender | ||||||||||||||||
Male | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||||
Female | -0.63*** | 0.057 | -0.63*** | 0.056 | -0.53*** | 0.123 | -0.60*** | 0.051 | -0.60*** | 0.051 | -0.50*** | 0.051 | ||||
Non-Binary+ | -1.9*** | 0.216 | -1.9*** | 0.218 | -1.7*** | 0.387 | -1.4*** | 0.208 | -1.4*** | 0.230 | -1.2*** | 0.202 | ||||
Ethnicity | ||||||||||||||||
White | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||||
Mixed | -0.33* | 0.133 | -0.27* | 0.127 | -0.34 | 0.278 | -0.12 | 0.128 | -0.14 | 0.124 | -0.12 | 0.126 | ||||
Black | 0.08 | 0.086 | 0.09 | 0.087 | 0.20 | 0.191 | 0.20** | 0.078 | 0.20* | 0.077 | 0.16* | 0.075 | ||||
Asian | -0.10 | 0.103 | 0.02 | 0.103 | 0.13 | 0.222 | 0.23* | 0.092 | 0.22* | 0.091 | 0.18* | 0.089 | ||||
Other | 0.03 | 0.229 | 0.06 | 0.224 | 0.16 | 0.513 | 0.22 | 0.189 | 0.22 | 0.184 | 0.22 | 0.193 | ||||
SES Quintile Groups | ||||||||||||||||
1 (Low SES) | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||||
2 | 0.03 | 0.094 | 0.00 | 0.093 | 0.05 | 0.146 | -0.04 | 0.083 | -0.03 | 0.081 | -0.03 | 0.080 | ||||
3 | 0.15 | 0.099 | 0.15 | 0.097 | 0.23 | 0.163 | 0.09 | 0.087 | 0.09 | 0.086 | 0.04 | 0.085 | ||||
4 | 0.26** | 0.090 | 0.27** | 0.089 | 0.33* | 0.140 | 0.16 | 0.082 | 0.16 | 0.082 | 0.09 | 0.081 | ||||
5 (High SES) | 0.30** | 0.094 | 0.30** | 0.094 | 0.36* | 0.142 | 0.21* | 0.086 | 0.21* | 0.085 | 0.12 | 0.084 | ||||
SES Quintile Groups * Gender | ||||||||||||||||
2 * Female | -0.09 | 0.175 | ||||||||||||||
3 * Female | -0.24 | 0.185 | ||||||||||||||
4 * Female | -0.05 | 0.170 | ||||||||||||||
5 (High SES) * Female | -0.01 | 0.172 | ||||||||||||||
2 * Non-Binary+ | -0.23 | 0.553 | ||||||||||||||
3 * Non-Binary+ | 0.46 | 0.697 | ||||||||||||||
4 * Non-Binary+ | 0.23 | 0.588 | ||||||||||||||
5 (High SES) * Non-Binary+ | -1.3* | 0.587 | ||||||||||||||
SES Quintile Groups * Ethnicity | ||||||||||||||||
2 * Mixed | 0.29 | 0.387 | ||||||||||||||
3 * Mixed | 0.26 | 0.363 | ||||||||||||||
4 * Mixed | -0.25 | 0.333 | ||||||||||||||
5 (High SES) * Mixed | 0.11 | 0.332 | ||||||||||||||
2 * Black | -0.02 | 0.246 | ||||||||||||||
3 * Black | -0.05 | 0.239 | ||||||||||||||
4 * Black | -0.23 | 0.287 | ||||||||||||||
5 (High SES) * Black | -0.35 | 0.303 | ||||||||||||||
2 * Asian | -0.11 | 0.220 | ||||||||||||||
3 * Asian | 0.07 | 0.258 | ||||||||||||||
4 * Asian | 0.04 | 0.387 | ||||||||||||||
5 (High SES) * Asian | -0.66 | 0.935 | ||||||||||||||
2 * Other | -0.06 | 0.551 | ||||||||||||||
3 * Other | 0.78 | 0.672 | ||||||||||||||
4 * Other | -0.15 | 0.664 | ||||||||||||||
5 (High SES) * Other | -0.08 | 0.949 | ||||||||||||||
Gender * Ethnicity | ||||||||||||||||
Female * Mixed | 0.01 | 0.249 | ||||||||||||||
Non-Binary+ * Mixed | -0.22 | 0.617 | ||||||||||||||
Female * Black | -0.09 | 0.161 | ||||||||||||||
Non-Binary+ * Black | 0.43 | 0.678 | ||||||||||||||
Female * Asian | -0.09 | 0.220 | ||||||||||||||
Non-Binary+ * Asian | -0.92 | 1.03 | ||||||||||||||
Female * Other | -0.36 | 0.454 | ||||||||||||||
Non-Binary+ * Other | 1.4* | 0.613 | ||||||||||||||
Social Provisions Scale | 0.90*** | 0.028 | 0.89*** | 0.075 | 0.83*** | 0.029 | ||||||||||
Gender * Social Provisions Scale | ||||||||||||||||
Female * Social Provisions Scale | 0.02 | 0.054 | ||||||||||||||
Non-Binary+ * Social Provisions Scale | -0.07 | 0.157 | ||||||||||||||
Ethnicity * Social Provisions Scale | ||||||||||||||||
Mixed * Social Provisions Scale | -0.17 | 0.109 | ||||||||||||||
Black * Social Provisions Scale | -0.12 | 0.074 | ||||||||||||||
Asian * Social Provisions Scale | -0.09 | 0.083 | ||||||||||||||
Other * Social Provisions Scale | 0.03 | 0.194 | ||||||||||||||
SES Quintile Groups * Social Provisions Scale | ||||||||||||||||
2 * Social Provisions Scale | 0.08 | 0.081 | ||||||||||||||
3 * Social Provisions Scale | 0.06 | 0.083 | ||||||||||||||
4 * Social Provisions Scale | -0.02 | 0.089 | ||||||||||||||
5 (High SES) * Social Provisions Scale | 0.03 | 0.086 | ||||||||||||||
Adverse Event Index | -0.35*** | 0.028 | ||||||||||||||
W1 Month of Interview | ||||||||||||||||
Sep 2021 | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Oct 2021 | 0.10 | 0.067 | 0.14* | 0.067 | 0.12 | 0.067 | 0.09 | 0.068 | 0.10 | 0.068 | 0.03 | 0.061 | 0.03 | 0.061 | 0.03 | 0.060 |
Nov 2021 | 0.41* | 0.193 | 0.42* | 0.185 | 0.37* | 0.184 | 0.36 | 0.188 | 0.37* | 0.183 | 0.38* | 0.169 | 0.39* | 0.169 | 0.38* | 0.165 |
Dec 2021 | 0.32* | 0.135 | 0.30* | 0.135 | 0.30* | 0.135 | 0.29* | 0.132 | 0.31* | 0.135 | 0.14 | 0.116 | 0.14 | 0.116 | 0.13 | 0.113 |
Jan 2022 | 0.49 | 0.250 | 0.50 | 0.260 | 0.51* | 0.257 | 0.48 | 0.246 | 0.47* | 0.241 | 0.47 | 0.260 | 0.47 | 0.262 | 0.43 | 0.256 |
Feb 2022 | -0.33 | 0.234 | -0.21 | 0.251 | -0.23 | 0.246 | -0.37 | 0.233 | -0.36 | 0.229 | -0.50* | 0.243 | -0.49* | 0.243 | -0.49* | 0.246 |
Mar 2022 | -0.12 | 0.093 | -0.10 | 0.095 | -0.10 | 0.096 | -0.12 | 0.093 | -0.12 | 0.093 | -0.14 | 0.084 | -0.14 | 0.084 | -0.13 | 0.084 |
Apr 2022 | -0.04 | 0.102 | 0.00 | 0.105 | -0.01 | 0.106 | -0.06 | 0.103 | -0.05 | 0.102 | -0.08 | 0.096 | -0.08 | 0.096 | -0.10 | 0.093 |
N | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | ||||||||
Residual DoF | 757 | 755 | 755 | 749 | 717 | 748 | 738 | 747 | ||||||||
Notes: All estimates are weighted and inference accounts for the complex survey design. | ||||||||||||||||
1 *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001 | ||||||||||||||||
2 SE = Standard Error |
Characteristic |
L1
|
L2
|
L3
|
L4
|
L5
|
L6
|
L7
|
L8
|
||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | |
(Intercept) | 6.8*** | 0.050 | 6.4*** | 0.043 | 6.3*** | 0.069 | 6.6*** | 0.081 | 6.6*** | 0.113 | 6.7*** | 0.075 | 6.6*** | 0.074 | 6.7*** | 0.073 |
Gender | ||||||||||||||||
Male | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||||
Female | -0.63*** | 0.057 | -0.63*** | 0.057 | -0.55*** | 0.123 | -0.60*** | 0.051 | -0.60*** | 0.051 | -0.51*** | 0.051 | ||||
Non-Binary+ | -1.9*** | 0.212 | -1.8*** | 0.214 | -1.7*** | 0.383 | -1.4*** | 0.204 | -1.4*** | 0.224 | -1.2*** | 0.199 | ||||
Ethnicity | ||||||||||||||||
White | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||||
Mixed | -0.33* | 0.135 | -0.27* | 0.127 | -0.32 | 0.279 | -0.12 | 0.129 | -0.14 | 0.125 | -0.11 | 0.126 | ||||
Black | 0.08 | 0.086 | 0.10 | 0.088 | 0.22 | 0.192 | 0.21** | 0.079 | 0.20* | 0.078 | 0.16* | 0.075 | ||||
Asian | -0.09 | 0.104 | 0.04 | 0.104 | 0.14 | 0.221 | 0.25** | 0.092 | 0.24** | 0.091 | 0.20* | 0.089 | ||||
Other | 0.01 | 0.230 | 0.05 | 0.225 | 0.15 | 0.515 | 0.21 | 0.190 | 0.21 | 0.185 | 0.21 | 0.194 | ||||
SES Quintile Groups | ||||||||||||||||
1 (Low SES) | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||||
2 | 0.03 | 0.095 | 0.00 | 0.093 | 0.05 | 0.147 | -0.03 | 0.083 | -0.03 | 0.082 | -0.03 | 0.080 | ||||
3 | 0.16 | 0.099 | 0.17 | 0.097 | 0.25 | 0.163 | 0.10 | 0.087 | 0.10 | 0.087 | 0.05 | 0.085 | ||||
4 | 0.27** | 0.089 | 0.28** | 0.089 | 0.32* | 0.140 | 0.16* | 0.082 | 0.16* | 0.082 | 0.10 | 0.080 | ||||
5 (High SES) | 0.33*** | 0.095 | 0.33*** | 0.095 | 0.39** | 0.141 | 0.24** | 0.086 | 0.24** | 0.086 | 0.15 | 0.085 | ||||
SES Quintile Groups * Gender | ||||||||||||||||
2 * Female | -0.08 | 0.175 | ||||||||||||||
3 * Female | -0.22 | 0.186 | ||||||||||||||
4 * Female | -0.03 | 0.170 | ||||||||||||||
5 (High SES) * Female | 0.01 | 0.175 | ||||||||||||||
2 * Non-Binary+ | -0.26 | 0.545 | ||||||||||||||
3 * Non-Binary+ | 0.46 | 0.690 | ||||||||||||||
4 * Non-Binary+ | 0.24 | 0.584 | ||||||||||||||
5 (High SES) * Non-Binary+ | -1.3* | 0.576 | ||||||||||||||
SES Quintile Groups * Ethnicity | ||||||||||||||||
2 * Mixed | 0.27 | 0.388 | ||||||||||||||
3 * Mixed | 0.22 | 0.365 | ||||||||||||||
4 * Mixed | -0.24 | 0.337 | ||||||||||||||
5 (High SES) * Mixed | 0.06 | 0.335 | ||||||||||||||
2 * Black | -0.04 | 0.243 | ||||||||||||||
3 * Black | -0.10 | 0.237 | ||||||||||||||
4 * Black | -0.25 | 0.289 | ||||||||||||||
5 (High SES) * Black | -0.35 | 0.308 | ||||||||||||||
2 * Asian | -0.13 | 0.220 | ||||||||||||||
3 * Asian | 0.05 | 0.258 | ||||||||||||||
4 * Asian | 0.05 | 0.395 | ||||||||||||||
5 (High SES) * Asian | -0.54 | 0.996 | ||||||||||||||
2 * Other | -0.05 | 0.555 | ||||||||||||||
3 * Other | 0.75 | 0.675 | ||||||||||||||
4 * Other | -0.14 | 0.660 | ||||||||||||||
5 (High SES) * Other | -0.08 | 0.981 | ||||||||||||||
Gender * Ethnicity | ||||||||||||||||
Female * Mixed | 0.01 | 0.252 | ||||||||||||||
Non-Binary+ * Mixed | -0.19 | 0.603 | ||||||||||||||
Female * Black | -0.06 | 0.161 | ||||||||||||||
Non-Binary+ * Black | 0.41 | 0.694 | ||||||||||||||
Female * Asian | -0.06 | 0.221 | ||||||||||||||
Non-Binary+ * Asian | -0.91 | 1.08 | ||||||||||||||
Female * Other | -0.37 | 0.454 | ||||||||||||||
Non-Binary+ * Other | 1.4* | 0.663 | ||||||||||||||
Social Provisions Scale | 0.90*** | 0.028 | 0.88*** | 0.075 | 0.83*** | 0.029 | ||||||||||
Gender * Social Provisions Scale | ||||||||||||||||
Female * Social Provisions Scale | 0.03 | 0.055 | ||||||||||||||
Non-Binary+ * Social Provisions Scale | -0.07 | 0.154 | ||||||||||||||
Ethnicity * Social Provisions Scale | ||||||||||||||||
Mixed * Social Provisions Scale | -0.18 | 0.108 | ||||||||||||||
Black * Social Provisions Scale | -0.11 | 0.073 | ||||||||||||||
Asian * Social Provisions Scale | -0.08 | 0.083 | ||||||||||||||
Other * Social Provisions Scale | 0.03 | 0.195 | ||||||||||||||
SES Quintile Groups * Social Provisions Scale | ||||||||||||||||
2 * Social Provisions Scale | 0.08 | 0.082 | ||||||||||||||
3 * Social Provisions Scale | 0.07 | 0.083 | ||||||||||||||
4 * Social Provisions Scale | -0.01 | 0.090 | ||||||||||||||
5 (High SES) * Social Provisions Scale | 0.03 | 0.088 | ||||||||||||||
Adverse Event Index | -0.35*** | 0.028 | ||||||||||||||
W2 Month of Survey | ||||||||||||||||
October 2022 | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — |
November 2022 | -0.07 | 0.059 | -0.03 | 0.061 | -0.04 | 0.061 | -0.09 | 0.059 | -0.08 | 0.059 | -0.09 | 0.053 | -0.09 | 0.053 | -0.11* | 0.052 |
December 2022 | -0.13 | 0.133 | -0.09 | 0.141 | -0.10 | 0.140 | -0.15 | 0.133 | -0.14 | 0.132 | -0.16 | 0.121 | -0.16 | 0.121 | -0.16 | 0.118 |
January 2023 | -0.31 | 0.222 | -0.21 | 0.222 | -0.20 | 0.226 | -0.31 | 0.227 | -0.31 | 0.225 | -0.38 | 0.201 | -0.37 | 0.200 | -0.39* | 0.188 |
February 2023 | 0.62** | 0.211 | 0.65** | 0.199 | 0.66** | 0.201 | 0.60** | 0.214 | 0.60** | 0.210 | 0.48* | 0.192 | 0.48* | 0.191 | 0.43* | 0.185 |
March 2023 | -0.16 | 0.212 | -0.09 | 0.213 | -0.07 | 0.211 | -0.16 | 0.209 | -0.17 | 0.208 | -0.09 | 0.203 | -0.10 | 0.203 | -0.09 | 0.204 |
April 2023 | -0.04 | 0.226 | -0.01 | 0.230 | -0.05 | 0.227 | -0.07 | 0.223 | -0.06 | 0.224 | 0.12 | 0.179 | 0.12 | 0.178 | 0.11 | 0.177 |
N | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | ||||||||
Residual DoF | 758 | 756 | 756 | 750 | 718 | 749 | 739 | 748 | ||||||||
Notes: All estimates are weighted and inference accounts for the complex survey design. | ||||||||||||||||
1 *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001 | ||||||||||||||||
2 SE = Standard Error |
Characteristic |
L1
|
L2
|
L3
|
L4
|
L5
|
L6
|
L7
|
L8
|
||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | |
(Intercept) | 3.3*** | 0.118 | 3.0*** | 0.106 | 2.9*** | 0.112 | 3.2*** | 0.126 | 3.1*** | 0.140 | 3.5*** | 0.138 | 3.5*** | 0.138 | 3.6*** | 0.138 |
Gender | ||||||||||||||||
Male | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||||
Female | -0.28*** | 0.046 | -0.29*** | 0.046 | -0.20 | 0.114 | -0.31*** | 0.046 | -0.31*** | 0.047 | -0.26*** | 0.046 | ||||
Non-Binary+ | -0.77*** | 0.169 | -0.75*** | 0.169 | -1.1** | 0.349 | -0.74*** | 0.165 | -0.70*** | 0.173 | -0.66*** | 0.162 | ||||
Wave 1 Wellbeing | 0.51*** | 0.015 | 0.53*** | 0.014 | 0.53*** | 0.014 | 0.51*** | 0.015 | 0.51*** | 0.015 | 0.47*** | 0.017 | 0.47*** | 0.017 | 0.45*** | 0.017 |
Ethnicity | ||||||||||||||||
White | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||||
Mixed | -0.17 | 0.098 | -0.15 | 0.098 | 0.01 | 0.242 | -0.13 | 0.099 | -0.12 | 0.100 | -0.13 | 0.098 | ||||
Black | -0.01 | 0.058 | 0.00 | 0.060 | 0.25 | 0.132 | 0.02 | 0.060 | 0.02 | 0.059 | -0.01 | 0.060 | ||||
Asian | 0.04 | 0.083 | 0.10 | 0.086 | 0.25 | 0.158 | 0.14 | 0.087 | 0.10 | 0.086 | 0.11 | 0.084 | ||||
Other | 0.18 | 0.177 | 0.19 | 0.176 | 0.30 | 0.386 | 0.23 | 0.173 | 0.25 | 0.166 | 0.23 | 0.169 | ||||
SES Quintile Groups | ||||||||||||||||
1 (Low SES) | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||||
2 | 0.10 | 0.075 | 0.09 | 0.074 | 0.14 | 0.124 | 0.09 | 0.074 | 0.08 | 0.073 | 0.09 | 0.073 | ||||
3 | 0.06 | 0.071 | 0.07 | 0.071 | 0.13 | 0.123 | 0.06 | 0.071 | 0.06 | 0.071 | 0.03 | 0.070 | ||||
4 | 0.13 | 0.076 | 0.15 | 0.077 | 0.22 | 0.125 | 0.13 | 0.076 | 0.13 | 0.077 | 0.09 | 0.075 | ||||
5 (High SES) | 0.15* | 0.075 | 0.17* | 0.078 | 0.21 | 0.114 | 0.16* | 0.077 | 0.16* | 0.077 | 0.11 | 0.076 | ||||
SES Quintile Groups * Gender | ||||||||||||||||
2 * Female | -0.07 | 0.150 | ||||||||||||||
3 * Female | -0.03 | 0.148 | ||||||||||||||
4 * Female | -0.07 | 0.154 | ||||||||||||||
5 (High SES) * Female | -0.05 | 0.144 | ||||||||||||||
2 * Non-Binary+ | -0.06 | 0.403 | ||||||||||||||
3 * Non-Binary+ | 0.68 | 0.424 | ||||||||||||||
4 * Non-Binary+ | 0.09 | 0.522 | ||||||||||||||
5 (High SES) * Non-Binary+ | 0.54 | 0.563 | ||||||||||||||
SES Quintile Groups * Ethnicity | ||||||||||||||||
2 * Mixed | -0.24 | 0.335 | ||||||||||||||
3 * Mixed | -0.27 | 0.244 | ||||||||||||||
4 * Mixed | -0.11 | 0.287 | ||||||||||||||
5 (High SES) * Mixed | 0.03 | 0.292 | ||||||||||||||
2 * Black | -0.13 | 0.165 | ||||||||||||||
3 * Black | -0.39* | 0.178 | ||||||||||||||
4 * Black | -0.20 | 0.200 | ||||||||||||||
5 (High SES) * Black | -0.42 | 0.216 | ||||||||||||||
2 * Asian | -0.09 | 0.208 | ||||||||||||||
3 * Asian | -0.13 | 0.234 | ||||||||||||||
4 * Asian | 0.17 | 0.213 | ||||||||||||||
5 (High SES) * Asian | 0.77 | 0.775 | ||||||||||||||
2 * Other | 0.85 | 0.437 | ||||||||||||||
3 * Other | 0.30 | 0.414 | ||||||||||||||
4 * Other | -0.93* | 0.448 | ||||||||||||||
5 (High SES) * Other | -0.77 | 0.700 | ||||||||||||||
Gender * Ethnicity | ||||||||||||||||
Female * Mixed | -0.13 | 0.212 | ||||||||||||||
Non-Binary+ * Mixed | 0.54 | 0.478 | ||||||||||||||
Female * Black | -0.16 | 0.114 | ||||||||||||||
Non-Binary+ * Black | -0.14 | 0.654 | ||||||||||||||
Female * Asian | -0.27 | 0.168 | ||||||||||||||
Non-Binary+ * Asian | 2.0** | 0.665 | ||||||||||||||
Female * Other | -0.35 | 0.332 | ||||||||||||||
Non-Binary+ * Other | 0.42 | 0.433 | ||||||||||||||
Social Provisions Scale | 0.19*** | 0.029 | 0.18** | 0.067 | 0.16*** | 0.029 | ||||||||||
Gender * Social Provisions Scale | ||||||||||||||||
Female * Social Provisions Scale | 0.03 | 0.055 | ||||||||||||||
Non-Binary+ * Social Provisions Scale | 0.09 | 0.126 | ||||||||||||||
Ethnicity * Social Provisions Scale | ||||||||||||||||
Mixed * Social Provisions Scale | 0.03 | 0.088 | ||||||||||||||
Black * Social Provisions Scale | 0.00 | 0.068 | ||||||||||||||
Asian * Social Provisions Scale | -0.21** | 0.078 | ||||||||||||||
Other * Social Provisions Scale | 0.11 | 0.174 | ||||||||||||||
SES Quintile Groups * Social Provisions Scale | ||||||||||||||||
2 * Social Provisions Scale | -0.06 | 0.073 | ||||||||||||||
3 * Social Provisions Scale | -0.01 | 0.076 | ||||||||||||||
4 * Social Provisions Scale | 0.03 | 0.084 | ||||||||||||||
5 (High SES) * Social Provisions Scale | 0.01 | 0.079 | ||||||||||||||
Adverse Event Index | -0.23*** | 0.024 | ||||||||||||||
W2 Month of Survey | ||||||||||||||||
October 2022 | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — |
November 2022 | -0.03 | 0.049 | -0.02 | 0.050 | -0.02 | 0.050 | -0.04 | 0.050 | -0.05 | 0.050 | -0.05 | 0.050 | -0.04 | 0.050 | -0.06 | 0.050 |
December 2022 | 0.24* | 0.103 | 0.26* | 0.105 | 0.26* | 0.104 | 0.23* | 0.103 | 0.23* | 0.102 | 0.23* | 0.103 | 0.23* | 0.102 | 0.22* | 0.100 |
January 2023 | 0.42 | 0.255 | 0.46 | 0.257 | 0.47 | 0.256 | 0.42 | 0.253 | 0.40 | 0.253 | 0.39 | 0.256 | 0.40 | 0.256 | 0.37 | 0.263 |
February 2023 | 0.31 | 0.208 | 0.32 | 0.204 | 0.33 | 0.206 | 0.31 | 0.210 | 0.29 | 0.210 | 0.31 | 0.203 | 0.31 | 0.201 | 0.29 | 0.205 |
March 2023 | 0.39** | 0.139 | 0.43** | 0.138 | 0.43** | 0.139 | 0.39** | 0.141 | 0.37** | 0.140 | 0.40** | 0.142 | 0.40** | 0.141 | 0.39** | 0.146 |
April 2023 | 0.16 | 0.183 | 0.16 | 0.185 | 0.16 | 0.185 | 0.14 | 0.184 | 0.16 | 0.183 | 0.18 | 0.180 | 0.18 | 0.180 | 0.18 | 0.183 |
N | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | ||||||||
Residual DoF | 757 | 755 | 755 | 749 | 717 | 748 | 738 | 747 | ||||||||
Notes: All estimates are weighted and inference accounts for the complex survey design. | ||||||||||||||||
1 *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001 | ||||||||||||||||
2 SE = Standard Error |
Characteristic |
P1
|
P2
|
P3
|
P4
|
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | |
(Intercept) | 6.7*** | 0.057 | 6.9*** | 0.103 | 6.9*** | 0.092 | 6.9*** | 0.106 |
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing | ||||||||
No | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Yes | -1.1*** | 0.062 | -1.0*** | 0.062 | -0.85*** | 0.055 | -1.0*** | 0.181 |
Gender | ||||||||
Male | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Female | -0.46*** | 0.057 | -0.47*** | 0.051 | -0.55*** | 0.061 | ||
Non-Binary+ | -1.5*** | 0.213 | -1.1*** | 0.205 | -1.2*** | 0.288 | ||
Ethnicity | ||||||||
White | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Mixed | -0.28* | 0.123 | -0.13 | 0.125 | -0.13 | 0.151 | ||
Black | 0.04 | 0.082 | 0.15* | 0.076 | 0.19* | 0.089 | ||
Asian | 0.00 | 0.102 | 0.19* | 0.092 | 0.12 | 0.111 | ||
Other | 0.09 | 0.219 | 0.22 | 0.187 | 0.22 | 0.227 | ||
Parental Education | ||||||||
Graduate | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Below Graduate | -0.07 | 0.066 | -0.05 | 0.059 | -0.09 | 0.071 | ||
No Quals | -0.22 | 0.124 | -0.09 | 0.115 | -0.10 | 0.144 | ||
Unknown | -0.09 | 0.303 | 0.14 | 0.331 | 0.18 | 0.401 | ||
Housing Tenure | ||||||||
Own House | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Other | -0.10 | 0.066 | -0.06 | 0.062 | 0.01 | 0.076 | ||
IDACI Quintile Group | ||||||||
1 (High Deprivation) | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
2 | 0.14 | 0.093 | 0.09 | 0.085 | 0.08 | 0.103 | ||
3 | 0.07 | 0.098 | 0.03 | 0.089 | 0.01 | 0.108 | ||
4 | 0.22* | 0.097 | 0.19* | 0.088 | 0.08 | 0.104 | ||
5 (Low Deprivation) | 0.27** | 0.103 | 0.21* | 0.093 | 0.24* | 0.112 | ||
Social Provisions Scale | 0.86*** | 0.028 | 0.86*** | 0.035 | ||||
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing * Gender | ||||||||
Yes * Female | 0.27* | 0.109 | ||||||
Yes * Non-Binary+ | 0.32 | 0.387 | ||||||
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing * Ethnicity | ||||||||
Yes * Mixed | 0.00 | 0.216 | ||||||
Yes * Black | -0.14 | 0.162 | ||||||
Yes * Asian | 0.23 | 0.184 | ||||||
Yes * Other | -0.05 | 0.416 | ||||||
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing * Parental Education | ||||||||
Yes * Below Graduate | 0.11 | 0.124 | ||||||
Yes * No Quals | 0.01 | 0.227 | ||||||
Yes * Unknown | -0.23 | 0.589 | ||||||
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing * Housing Tenure | ||||||||
Yes * Other | -0.22 | 0.126 | ||||||
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing * IDACI Quintile Group | ||||||||
Yes * 2 | 0.06 | 0.169 | ||||||
Yes * 3 | 0.08 | 0.189 | ||||||
Yes * 4 | 0.35 | 0.183 | ||||||
Yes * 5 (Low Deprivation) | -0.08 | 0.199 | ||||||
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing * Social Provisions Scale | ||||||||
Yes * Social Provisions Scale | -0.02 | 0.052 | ||||||
W1 Month of Interview | ||||||||
Sep 2021 | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Oct 2021 | 0.12 | 0.067 | 0.07 | 0.067 | 0.02 | 0.060 | 0.02 | 0.060 |
Nov 2021 | 0.37* | 0.185 | 0.29 | 0.184 | 0.33 | 0.169 | 0.33 | 0.167 |
Dec 2021 | 0.25 | 0.132 | 0.23 | 0.132 | 0.10 | 0.114 | 0.10 | 0.112 |
Jan 2022 | 0.48 | 0.254 | 0.45 | 0.234 | 0.45 | 0.256 | 0.43 | 0.258 |
Feb 2022 | -0.39 | 0.235 | -0.52* | 0.225 | -0.62** | 0.233 | -0.61* | 0.236 |
Mar 2022 | -0.08 | 0.093 | -0.11 | 0.092 | -0.13 | 0.083 | -0.12 | 0.083 |
Apr 2022 | -0.01 | 0.103 | -0.07 | 0.102 | -0.09 | 0.094 | -0.08 | 0.094 |
N | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | ||||
Residual DoF | 758 | 744 | 743 | 728 | ||||
Notes: All estimates are weighted and inference accounts for the complex survey design. | ||||||||
1 *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001 | ||||||||
2 SE = Standard Error |
Characteristic |
P1
|
P2
|
P3
|
P4
|
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | |
(Intercept) | 6.9*** | 0.041 | 7.0*** | 0.093 | 6.9*** | 0.089 | 7.0*** | 0.104 |
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing | ||||||||
No | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Yes | -1.4*** | 0.059 | -1.3*** | 0.058 | -1.2*** | 0.057 | -1.3*** | 0.193 |
Gender | ||||||||
Male | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Female | -0.40*** | 0.056 | -0.41*** | 0.053 | -0.47*** | 0.063 | ||
Non-Binary+ | -1.2*** | 0.184 | -0.98*** | 0.175 | -1.2*** | 0.248 | ||
Ethnicity | ||||||||
White | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Mixed | -0.29* | 0.118 | -0.19 | 0.120 | -0.15 | 0.144 | ||
Black | -0.06 | 0.073 | 0.00 | 0.072 | 0.05 | 0.085 | ||
Asian | 0.10 | 0.104 | 0.22* | 0.103 | 0.14 | 0.127 | ||
Other | 0.25 | 0.194 | 0.33 | 0.177 | 0.45* | 0.197 | ||
Parental Education | ||||||||
Graduate | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Below Graduate | -0.03 | 0.064 | -0.01 | 0.059 | 0.01 | 0.072 | ||
No Quals | 0.14 | 0.109 | 0.22* | 0.107 | 0.30* | 0.131 | ||
Unknown | -0.10 | 0.318 | 0.04 | 0.342 | -0.06 | 0.418 | ||
Housing Tenure | ||||||||
Own House | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Other | -0.23*** | 0.060 | -0.21*** | 0.059 | -0.20** | 0.074 | ||
IDACI Quintile Group | ||||||||
1 (High Deprivation) | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
2 | 0.19* | 0.088 | 0.16 | 0.084 | 0.13 | 0.102 | ||
3 | 0.22* | 0.092 | 0.19* | 0.087 | 0.13 | 0.105 | ||
4 | 0.26** | 0.091 | 0.24** | 0.087 | 0.21* | 0.105 | ||
5 (Low Deprivation) | 0.23* | 0.094 | 0.19* | 0.088 | 0.19 | 0.109 | ||
Social Provisions Scale | 0.55*** | 0.029 | 0.56*** | 0.039 | ||||
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing * Gender | ||||||||
Yes * Female | 0.22 | 0.117 | ||||||
Yes * Non-Binary+ | 0.41 | 0.344 | ||||||
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing * Ethnicity | ||||||||
Yes * Mixed | -0.13 | 0.281 | ||||||
Yes * Black | -0.19 | 0.168 | ||||||
Yes * Asian | 0.28 | 0.209 | ||||||
Yes * Other | -0.36 | 0.415 | ||||||
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing * Parental Education | ||||||||
Yes * Below Graduate | -0.08 | 0.124 | ||||||
Yes * No Quals | -0.31 | 0.222 | ||||||
Yes * Unknown | 0.50 | 0.494 | ||||||
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing * Housing Tenure | ||||||||
Yes * Other | -0.02 | 0.126 | ||||||
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing * IDACI Quintile Group | ||||||||
Yes * 2 | 0.10 | 0.169 | ||||||
Yes * 3 | 0.19 | 0.186 | ||||||
Yes * 4 | 0.09 | 0.187 | ||||||
Yes * 5 (Low Deprivation) | -0.01 | 0.196 | ||||||
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing * Social Provisions Scale | ||||||||
Yes * Social Provisions Scale | -0.03 | 0.058 | ||||||
W2 Month of Survey | ||||||||
October 2022 | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — |
November 2022 | -0.04 | 0.056 | -0.09 | 0.055 | -0.09 | 0.053 | -0.09 | 0.053 |
December 2022 | 0.13 | 0.126 | 0.09 | 0.122 | 0.09 | 0.116 | 0.09 | 0.115 |
January 2023 | 0.21 | 0.271 | 0.13 | 0.270 | 0.10 | 0.268 | 0.10 | 0.268 |
February 2023 | 0.47 | 0.243 | 0.41 | 0.256 | 0.35 | 0.224 | 0.36 | 0.222 |
March 2023 | 0.27 | 0.181 | 0.21 | 0.180 | 0.27 | 0.182 | 0.27 | 0.180 |
April 2023 | 0.12 | 0.208 | 0.07 | 0.202 | 0.18 | 0.180 | 0.18 | 0.180 |
N | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | ||||
Residual DoF | 759 | 745 | 744 | 729 | ||||
Notes: All estimates are weighted and inference accounts for the complex survey design. | ||||||||
1 *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001 | ||||||||
2 SE = Standard Error |
Characteristic |
P1
|
P2
|
P3
|
P4
|
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | |
(Intercept) | 3.6*** | 0.121 | 3.7*** | 0.150 | 4.0*** | 0.158 | 4.0*** | 0.163 |
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing | ||||||||
No | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Yes | -0.85*** | 0.055 | -0.81*** | 0.055 | -0.81*** | 0.055 | -0.86*** | 0.179 |
Wave 1 Wellbeing | 0.48*** | 0.015 | 0.46*** | 0.015 | 0.42*** | 0.017 | 0.42*** | 0.017 |
Gender | ||||||||
Male | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Female | -0.19*** | 0.046 | -0.21*** | 0.046 | -0.23*** | 0.054 | ||
Non-Binary+ | -0.54** | 0.167 | -0.53** | 0.164 | -0.67* | 0.269 | ||
Ethnicity | ||||||||
White | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Mixed | -0.15 | 0.095 | -0.13 | 0.097 | -0.08 | 0.127 | ||
Black | -0.09 | 0.060 | -0.06 | 0.061 | -0.03 | 0.072 | ||
Asian | 0.09 | 0.089 | 0.13 | 0.090 | 0.08 | 0.110 | ||
Other | 0.22 | 0.172 | 0.25 | 0.169 | 0.37 | 0.195 | ||
Parental Education | ||||||||
Graduate | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Below Graduate | 0.03 | 0.052 | 0.03 | 0.051 | 0.07 | 0.062 | ||
No Quals | 0.24** | 0.087 | 0.26** | 0.088 | 0.34*** | 0.103 | ||
Unknown | -0.07 | 0.264 | -0.03 | 0.273 | -0.16 | 0.332 | ||
Housing Tenure | ||||||||
Own House | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Other | -0.18*** | 0.053 | -0.18*** | 0.053 | -0.21** | 0.063 | ||
IDACI Quintile Group | ||||||||
1 (High Deprivation) | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
2 | 0.12 | 0.075 | 0.12 | 0.075 | 0.09 | 0.090 | ||
3 | 0.19* | 0.080 | 0.18* | 0.079 | 0.12 | 0.094 | ||
4 | 0.15* | 0.077 | 0.15* | 0.077 | 0.17 | 0.091 | ||
5 (Low Deprivation) | 0.08 | 0.078 | 0.08 | 0.078 | 0.06 | 0.097 | ||
Social Provisions Scale | 0.19*** | 0.029 | 0.20*** | 0.037 | ||||
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing * Gender | ||||||||
Yes * Female | 0.10 | 0.107 | ||||||
Yes * Non-Binary+ | 0.28 | 0.336 | ||||||
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing * Ethnicity | ||||||||
Yes * Mixed | -0.14 | 0.265 | ||||||
Yes * Black | -0.14 | 0.149 | ||||||
Yes * Asian | 0.16 | 0.189 | ||||||
Yes * Other | -0.33 | 0.374 | ||||||
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing * Parental Education | ||||||||
Yes * Below Graduate | -0.13 | 0.109 | ||||||
Yes * No Quals | -0.31 | 0.199 | ||||||
Yes * Unknown | 0.64 | 0.445 | ||||||
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing * Housing Tenure | ||||||||
Yes * Other | 0.09 | 0.112 | ||||||
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing * IDACI Quintile Group | ||||||||
Yes * 2 | 0.08 | 0.155 | ||||||
Yes * 3 | 0.16 | 0.166 | ||||||
Yes * 4 | -0.06 | 0.174 | ||||||
Yes * 5 (Low Deprivation) | 0.03 | 0.172 | ||||||
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing * Social Provisions Scale | ||||||||
Yes * Social Provisions Scale | -0.02 | 0.053 | ||||||
W1 Month of Interview | ||||||||
Sep 2021 | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Oct 2021 | 0.07 | 0.057 | 0.05 | 0.055 | 0.05 | 0.055 | 0.05 | 0.055 |
Nov 2021 | 0.29* | 0.113 | 0.27* | 0.116 | 0.29* | 0.114 | 0.29* | 0.115 |
Dec 2021 | 0.30* | 0.118 | 0.29* | 0.119 | 0.27* | 0.117 | 0.28* | 0.116 |
Jan 2022 | 0.15 | 0.160 | 0.17 | 0.150 | 0.19 | 0.154 | 0.19 | 0.152 |
Feb 2022 | 0.32 | 0.340 | 0.29 | 0.334 | 0.25 | 0.346 | 0.25 | 0.345 |
Mar 2022 | -0.05 | 0.078 | -0.06 | 0.078 | -0.07 | 0.078 | -0.07 | 0.078 |
Apr 2022 | 0.01 | 0.080 | -0.02 | 0.080 | -0.02 | 0.080 | -0.02 | 0.080 |
W2 Month of Survey | ||||||||
October 2022 | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — |
November 2022 | -0.04 | 0.049 | -0.06 | 0.049 | -0.06 | 0.049 | -0.06 | 0.049 |
December 2022 | 0.19 | 0.103 | 0.17 | 0.102 | 0.16 | 0.101 | 0.16 | 0.101 |
January 2023 | 0.36 | 0.244 | 0.32 | 0.242 | 0.30 | 0.244 | 0.29 | 0.244 |
February 2023 | 0.21 | 0.217 | 0.18 | 0.220 | 0.18 | 0.212 | 0.18 | 0.211 |
March 2023 | 0.32* | 0.140 | 0.29* | 0.142 | 0.30* | 0.145 | 0.30* | 0.144 |
April 2023 | 0.12 | 0.180 | 0.10 | 0.179 | 0.13 | 0.175 | 0.13 | 0.174 |
N | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | ||||
Residual DoF | 751 | 737 | 736 | 721 | ||||
Notes: All estimates are weighted and inference accounts for the complex survey design. | ||||||||
1 *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001 | ||||||||
2 SE = Standard Error |
Characteristic |
E1
|
E2
|
E3
|
E4
|
E5
|
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | |
(Intercept) | 7.0*** | 0.064 | 7.2*** | 0.107 | 7.0*** | 0.101 | 7.1*** | 0.099 | 7.0*** | 0.143 |
Adverse Event Tercile Groups | ||||||||||
Low | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Medium | -0.58*** | 0.066 | -0.53*** | 0.066 | -0.36*** | 0.062 | -0.30*** | 0.062 | 0.15 | 0.183 |
High | -1.4*** | 0.070 | -1.3*** | 0.070 | -0.85*** | 0.068 | -0.68*** | 0.069 | -0.70*** | 0.189 |
Gender | ||||||||||
Male | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Female | -0.48*** | 0.056 | -0.50*** | 0.051 | -0.41*** | 0.051 | -0.43*** | 0.081 | ||
Non-Binary+ | -1.6*** | 0.204 | -1.3*** | 0.200 | -1.0*** | 0.200 | -0.91* | 0.409 | ||
Ethnicity | ||||||||||
White | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Mixed | -0.23 | 0.126 | -0.11 | 0.128 | -0.12 | 0.125 | -0.12 | 0.145 | ||
Black | 0.03 | 0.086 | 0.14 | 0.078 | 0.12 | 0.074 | 0.15 | 0.087 | ||
Asian | -0.03 | 0.100 | 0.17 | 0.091 | 0.15 | 0.090 | 0.11 | 0.107 | ||
Other | 0.08 | 0.220 | 0.20 | 0.190 | 0.20 | 0.187 | 0.24 | 0.218 | ||
Parental Education | ||||||||||
Graduate | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Below Graduate | -0.02 | 0.064 | -0.01 | 0.058 | -0.04 | 0.058 | 0.03 | 0.089 | ||
No Quals | -0.16 | 0.126 | -0.04 | 0.116 | -0.10 | 0.114 | 0.07 | 0.189 | ||
Unknown | -0.01 | 0.314 | 0.21 | 0.337 | 0.11 | 0.333 | 0.32 | 0.607 | ||
Housing Tenure | ||||||||||
Own House | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Other | -0.09 | 0.067 | -0.06 | 0.063 | -0.04 | 0.062 | 0.05 | 0.106 | ||
IDACI Quintile Group | ||||||||||
1 (High Deprivation) | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
2 | 0.05 | 0.091 | 0.03 | 0.084 | 0.06 | 0.084 | 0.18 | 0.141 | ||
3 | -0.02 | 0.098 | -0.03 | 0.090 | 0.00 | 0.088 | 0.00 | 0.158 | ||
4 | 0.17 | 0.096 | 0.15 | 0.088 | 0.17 | 0.087 | 0.28* | 0.138 | ||
5 (Low Deprivation) | 0.14 | 0.100 | 0.12 | 0.093 | 0.17 | 0.091 | 0.20 | 0.148 | ||
Social Provisions Scale | 0.83*** | 0.029 | 0.81*** | 0.029 | 0.88*** | 0.062 | ||||
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing | ||||||||||
No | — | — | — | — | ||||||
Yes | -0.72*** | 0.055 | -0.71*** | 0.064 | ||||||
Adverse Event Tercile Groups * Gender | ||||||||||
Medium * Female | -0.08 | 0.111 | ||||||||
High * Female | 0.14 | 0.124 | ||||||||
Medium * Non-Binary+ | -0.14 | 0.572 | ||||||||
High * Non-Binary+ | -0.16 | 0.464 | ||||||||
Ethnicity * Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing | ||||||||||
Mixed * Yes | -0.03 | 0.211 | ||||||||
Black * Yes | -0.12 | 0.151 | ||||||||
Asian * Yes | 0.15 | 0.171 | ||||||||
Other * Yes | -0.16 | 0.411 | ||||||||
Adverse Event Tercile Groups * Parental Education | ||||||||||
Medium * Below Graduate | 0.00 | 0.130 | ||||||||
High * Below Graduate | -0.20 | 0.136 | ||||||||
Medium * No Quals | -0.26 | 0.234 | ||||||||
High * No Quals | -0.31 | 0.283 | ||||||||
Medium * Unknown | -0.93 | 0.747 | ||||||||
High * Unknown | 0.48 | 0.749 | ||||||||
Adverse Event Tercile Groups * Housing Tenure | ||||||||||
Medium * Other | -0.26 | 0.147 | ||||||||
High * Other | -0.04 | 0.150 | ||||||||
Adverse Event Tercile Groups * IDACI Quintile Group | ||||||||||
Medium * 2 | -0.36 | 0.197 | ||||||||
High * 2 | -0.12 | 0.194 | ||||||||
Medium * 3 | -0.25 | 0.210 | ||||||||
High * 3 | 0.18 | 0.208 | ||||||||
Medium * 4 | -0.57** | 0.192 | ||||||||
High * 4 | 0.12 | 0.193 | ||||||||
Medium * 5 (Low Deprivation) | -0.25 | 0.202 | ||||||||
High * 5 (Low Deprivation) | 0.07 | 0.211 | ||||||||
Adverse Event Tercile Groups * Social Provisions Scale | ||||||||||
Medium * Social Provisions Scale | -0.03 | 0.075 | ||||||||
High * Social Provisions Scale | -0.14 | 0.072 | ||||||||
W1 Month of Interview | ||||||||||
Sep 2021 | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Oct 2021 | 0.10 | 0.065 | 0.07 | 0.065 | 0.02 | 0.059 | 0.02 | 0.059 | 0.03 | 0.059 |
Nov 2021 | 0.42* | 0.186 | 0.37* | 0.183 | 0.39* | 0.168 | 0.35* | 0.168 | 0.36* | 0.165 |
Dec 2021 | 0.26* | 0.126 | 0.24 | 0.125 | 0.12 | 0.113 | 0.09 | 0.111 | 0.10 | 0.109 |
Jan 2022 | 0.45 | 0.262 | 0.44 | 0.246 | 0.45 | 0.253 | 0.43 | 0.255 | 0.42 | 0.255 |
Feb 2022 | -0.24 | 0.263 | -0.38 | 0.250 | -0.50* | 0.247 | -0.60* | 0.238 | -0.60* | 0.240 |
Mar 2022 | -0.11 | 0.094 | -0.13 | 0.092 | -0.15 | 0.084 | -0.13 | 0.083 | -0.13 | 0.083 |
Apr 2022 | -0.06 | 0.099 | -0.11 | 0.098 | -0.11 | 0.093 | -0.11 | 0.092 | -0.10 | 0.091 |
N | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | |||||
Residual DoF | 757 | 743 | 742 | 741 | 715 | |||||
Notes: All estimates are weighted and inference accounts for the complex survey design. | ||||||||||
1 *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001 | ||||||||||
2 SE = Standard Error |
Characteristic |
E1
|
E2
|
E3
|
E4
|
E5
|
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | |
(Intercept) | 7.0*** | 0.049 | 7.2*** | 0.098 | 7.0*** | 0.095 | 7.2*** | 0.092 | 7.2*** | 0.123 |
Adverse Event Tercile Groups | ||||||||||
Low | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Medium | -0.55*** | 0.064 | -0.50*** | 0.063 | -0.39*** | 0.063 | -0.31*** | 0.061 | -0.32 | 0.190 |
High | -1.3*** | 0.066 | -1.1*** | 0.066 | -0.88*** | 0.067 | -0.64*** | 0.067 | -0.61*** | 0.182 |
Gender | ||||||||||
Male | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Female | -0.48*** | 0.054 | -0.49*** | 0.052 | -0.35*** | 0.053 | -0.35*** | 0.080 | ||
Non-Binary+ | -1.4*** | 0.174 | -1.2*** | 0.167 | -0.92*** | 0.171 | -0.92** | 0.346 | ||
Ethnicity | ||||||||||
White | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Mixed | -0.24 | 0.124 | -0.16 | 0.126 | -0.18 | 0.120 | -0.15 | 0.139 | ||
Black | -0.06 | 0.078 | 0.01 | 0.075 | -0.02 | 0.072 | 0.02 | 0.083 | ||
Asian | 0.09 | 0.101 | 0.22* | 0.100 | 0.18 | 0.099 | 0.09 | 0.121 | ||
Other | 0.23 | 0.202 | 0.31 | 0.186 | 0.32 | 0.177 | 0.43* | 0.195 | ||
Parental Education | ||||||||||
Graduate | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Below Graduate | 0.03 | 0.064 | 0.04 | 0.060 | -0.01 | 0.059 | -0.05 | 0.090 | ||
No Quals | 0.22* | 0.112 | 0.30** | 0.109 | 0.20 | 0.106 | 0.37* | 0.160 | ||
Unknown | 0.02 | 0.325 | 0.15 | 0.347 | 0.01 | 0.345 | -0.18 | 0.338 | ||
Housing Tenure | ||||||||||
Own House | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Other | -0.24*** | 0.062 | -0.22*** | 0.061 | -0.18** | 0.059 | -0.16 | 0.096 | ||
IDACI Quintile Group | ||||||||||
1 (High Deprivation) | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
2 | 0.10 | 0.086 | 0.09 | 0.083 | 0.13 | 0.083 | 0.16 | 0.126 | ||
3 | 0.13 | 0.093 | 0.11 | 0.088 | 0.17* | 0.086 | 0.16 | 0.135 | ||
4 | 0.22* | 0.093 | 0.20* | 0.090 | 0.23** | 0.087 | 0.17 | 0.126 | ||
5 (Low Deprivation) | 0.09 | 0.095 | 0.07 | 0.091 | 0.14 | 0.087 | 0.06 | 0.137 | ||
Social Provisions Scale | 0.53*** | 0.030 | 0.50*** | 0.030 | 0.57*** | 0.063 | ||||
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing | ||||||||||
No | — | — | — | — | ||||||
Yes | -1.1*** | 0.057 | -1.0*** | 0.065 | ||||||
Adverse Event Tercile Groups * Gender | ||||||||||
Medium * Female | -0.05 | 0.124 | ||||||||
High * Female | 0.01 | 0.131 | ||||||||
Medium * Non-Binary+ | 0.37 | 0.472 | ||||||||
High * Non-Binary+ | -0.23 | 0.389 | ||||||||
Ethnicity * Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing | ||||||||||
Mixed * Yes | -0.12 | 0.281 | ||||||||
Black * Yes | -0.17 | 0.150 | ||||||||
Asian * Yes | 0.31 | 0.192 | ||||||||
Other * Yes | -0.43 | 0.410 | ||||||||
Adverse Event Tercile Groups * Parental Education | ||||||||||
Medium * Below Graduate | 0.09 | 0.132 | ||||||||
High * Below Graduate | 0.03 | 0.139 | ||||||||
Medium * No Quals | -0.14 | 0.229 | ||||||||
High * No Quals | -0.39 | 0.255 | ||||||||
Medium * Unknown | 0.17 | 0.805 | ||||||||
High * Unknown | 0.38 | 0.599 | ||||||||
Adverse Event Tercile Groups * Housing Tenure | ||||||||||
Medium * Other | 0.10 | 0.155 | ||||||||
High * Other | -0.15 | 0.143 | ||||||||
Adverse Event Tercile Groups * IDACI Quintile Group | ||||||||||
Medium * 2 | -0.11 | 0.196 | ||||||||
High * 2 | -0.05 | 0.198 | ||||||||
Medium * 3 | -0.10 | 0.204 | ||||||||
High * 3 | 0.09 | 0.191 | ||||||||
Medium * 4 | 0.00 | 0.197 | ||||||||
High * 4 | 0.13 | 0.183 | ||||||||
Medium * 5 (Low Deprivation) | 0.11 | 0.216 | ||||||||
High * 5 (Low Deprivation) | 0.13 | 0.208 | ||||||||
Adverse Event Tercile Groups * Social Provisions Scale | ||||||||||
Medium * Social Provisions Scale | -0.06 | 0.082 | ||||||||
High * Social Provisions Scale | -0.11 | 0.072 | ||||||||
W2 Month of Survey | ||||||||||
October 2022 | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — |
November 2022 | -0.05 | 0.057 | -0.10 | 0.056 | -0.10 | 0.055 | -0.10 | 0.053 | -0.09 | 0.053 |
December 2022 | 0.19 | 0.124 | 0.15 | 0.118 | 0.15 | 0.114 | 0.09 | 0.113 | 0.10 | 0.113 |
January 2023 | 0.35 | 0.310 | 0.25 | 0.301 | 0.21 | 0.296 | 0.12 | 0.279 | 0.10 | 0.273 |
February 2023 | 0.60* | 0.234 | 0.53* | 0.246 | 0.48* | 0.221 | 0.35 | 0.224 | 0.34 | 0.221 |
March 2023 | 0.39* | 0.186 | 0.31 | 0.187 | 0.35 | 0.185 | 0.28 | 0.185 | 0.27 | 0.183 |
April 2023 | 0.17 | 0.216 | 0.13 | 0.210 | 0.23 | 0.188 | 0.19 | 0.184 | 0.17 | 0.185 |
N | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | |||||
Residual DoF | 758 | 744 | 743 | 742 | 716 | |||||
Notes: All estimates are weighted and inference accounts for the complex survey design. | ||||||||||
1 *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001 | ||||||||||
2 SE = Standard Error |
Characteristic |
E1
|
E2
|
E3
|
E4
|
E5
|
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | Beta1 | SE2 | |
(Intercept) | 3.5*** | 0.126 | 3.7*** | 0.154 | 4.2*** | 0.163 | 4.2*** | 0.163 | 4.3*** | 0.178 |
Adverse Event Tercile Groups | ||||||||||
Low | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Medium | -0.26*** | 0.056 | -0.25*** | 0.056 | -0.19*** | 0.054 | -0.19*** | 0.054 | -0.37* | 0.167 |
High | -0.59*** | 0.060 | -0.54*** | 0.060 | -0.36*** | 0.060 | -0.36*** | 0.060 | -0.33 | 0.171 |
Wave 1 Wellbeing | 0.49*** | 0.015 | 0.48*** | 0.015 | 0.41*** | 0.017 | 0.41*** | 0.017 | 0.41*** | 0.017 |
Gender | ||||||||||
Male | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Female | -0.24*** | 0.045 | -0.18*** | 0.046 | -0.18*** | 0.046 | -0.17* | 0.070 | ||
Non-Binary+ | -0.69*** | 0.162 | -0.50** | 0.162 | -0.50** | 0.162 | -0.52 | 0.315 | ||
Ethnicity | ||||||||||
White | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Mixed | -0.12 | 0.098 | -0.13 | 0.098 | -0.13 | 0.098 | -0.10 | 0.126 | ||
Black | -0.08 | 0.061 | -0.07 | 0.061 | -0.07 | 0.061 | -0.04 | 0.071 | ||
Asian | 0.09 | 0.088 | 0.11 | 0.089 | 0.11 | 0.089 | 0.03 | 0.106 | ||
Other | 0.21 | 0.177 | 0.25 | 0.169 | 0.25 | 0.169 | 0.34 | 0.194 | ||
Parental Education | ||||||||||
Graduate | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Below Graduate | 0.06 | 0.053 | 0.03 | 0.051 | 0.03 | 0.051 | -0.03 | 0.077 | ||
No Quals | 0.30*** | 0.088 | 0.25** | 0.088 | 0.25** | 0.088 | 0.34* | 0.135 | ||
Unknown | 0.01 | 0.263 | -0.04 | 0.276 | -0.04 | 0.276 | -0.30 | 0.245 | ||
Housing Tenure | ||||||||||
Own House | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
Other | -0.20*** | 0.054 | -0.17** | 0.053 | -0.17** | 0.053 | -0.18* | 0.081 | ||
IDACI Quintile Group | ||||||||||
1 (High Deprivation) | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||
2 | 0.07 | 0.074 | 0.10 | 0.075 | 0.10 | 0.075 | 0.08 | 0.111 | ||
3 | 0.13 | 0.081 | 0.17* | 0.079 | 0.17* | 0.079 | 0.16 | 0.117 | ||
4 | 0.12 | 0.079 | 0.15 | 0.077 | 0.15 | 0.077 | 0.05 | 0.114 | ||
5 (Low Deprivation) | 0.00 | 0.079 | 0.05 | 0.078 | 0.05 | 0.078 | -0.03 | 0.119 | ||
Social Provisions Scale | 0.17*** | 0.029 | 0.17*** | 0.029 | 0.21*** | 0.053 | ||||
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing | ||||||||||
No | — | — | — | — | — | — | ||||
Yes | -0.76*** | 0.055 | -0.76*** | 0.055 | -0.74*** | 0.062 | ||||
Adverse Event Tercile Groups * Gender | ||||||||||
Medium * Female | -0.02 | 0.111 | ||||||||
High * Female | -0.04 | 0.121 | ||||||||
Medium * Non-Binary+ | 0.37 | 0.434 | ||||||||
High * Non-Binary+ | -0.20 | 0.359 | ||||||||
Ethnicity * Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing | ||||||||||
Mixed * Yes | -0.12 | 0.266 | ||||||||
Black * Yes | -0.13 | 0.135 | ||||||||
Asian * Yes | 0.23 | 0.174 | ||||||||
Other * Yes | -0.35 | 0.374 | ||||||||
Adverse Event Tercile Groups * Parental Education | ||||||||||
Medium * Below Graduate | 0.09 | 0.118 | ||||||||
High * Below Graduate | 0.11 | 0.123 | ||||||||
Medium * No Quals | -0.04 | 0.207 | ||||||||
High * No Quals | -0.24 | 0.208 | ||||||||
Medium * Unknown | 0.47 | 0.647 | ||||||||
High * Unknown | 0.20 | 0.440 | ||||||||
Adverse Event Tercile Groups * Housing Tenure | ||||||||||
Medium * Other | 0.22 | 0.136 | ||||||||
High * Other | -0.14 | 0.126 | ||||||||
Adverse Event Tercile Groups * IDACI Quintile Group | ||||||||||
Medium * 2 | 0.03 | 0.174 | ||||||||
High * 2 | 0.02 | 0.183 | ||||||||
Medium * 3 | -0.01 | 0.176 | ||||||||
High * 3 | 0.03 | 0.174 | ||||||||
Medium * 4 | 0.23 | 0.177 | ||||||||
High * 4 | 0.07 | 0.170 | ||||||||
Medium * 5 (Low Deprivation) | 0.18 | 0.193 | ||||||||
High * 5 (Low Deprivation) | 0.08 | 0.185 | ||||||||
Adverse Event Tercile Groups * Social Provisions Scale | ||||||||||
Medium * Social Provisions Scale | -0.04 | 0.073 | ||||||||
High * Social Provisions Scale | -0.05 | 0.061 | ||||||||
W1 Month of Interview | ||||||||||
Sep 2021 | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Oct 2021 | 0.06 | 0.058 | 0.05 | 0.056 | 0.05 | 0.055 | 0.05 | 0.055 | 0.04 | 0.055 |
Nov 2021 | 0.32** | 0.114 | 0.32** | 0.118 | 0.31** | 0.116 | 0.31** | 0.116 | 0.30* | 0.118 |
Dec 2021 | 0.31** | 0.111 | 0.31** | 0.112 | 0.27* | 0.114 | 0.27* | 0.114 | 0.26* | 0.113 |
Jan 2022 | 0.13 | 0.166 | 0.17 | 0.155 | 0.19 | 0.156 | 0.19 | 0.156 | 0.16 | 0.159 |
Feb 2022 | 0.44 | 0.347 | 0.41 | 0.339 | 0.26 | 0.348 | 0.26 | 0.348 | 0.27 | 0.350 |
Mar 2022 | -0.06 | 0.078 | -0.08 | 0.078 | -0.07 | 0.077 | -0.07 | 0.077 | -0.08 | 0.077 |
Apr 2022 | -0.02 | 0.080 | -0.04 | 0.080 | -0.04 | 0.080 | -0.04 | 0.080 | -0.04 | 0.079 |
W2 Month of Survey | ||||||||||
October 2022 | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — |
November 2022 | -0.04 | 0.049 | -0.06 | 0.049 | -0.06 | 0.049 | -0.06 | 0.049 | -0.06 | 0.048 |
December 2022 | 0.23* | 0.102 | 0.21* | 0.101 | 0.16 | 0.100 | 0.16 | 0.100 | 0.16 | 0.099 |
January 2023 | 0.45 | 0.269 | 0.40 | 0.264 | 0.30 | 0.253 | 0.30 | 0.253 | 0.29 | 0.249 |
February 2023 | 0.29 | 0.212 | 0.25 | 0.215 | 0.18 | 0.213 | 0.18 | 0.213 | 0.17 | 0.210 |
March 2023 | 0.39** | 0.144 | 0.35* | 0.146 | 0.30* | 0.148 | 0.30* | 0.148 | 0.30* | 0.147 |
April 2023 | 0.15 | 0.185 | 0.13 | 0.183 | 0.14 | 0.177 | 0.14 | 0.177 | 0.13 | 0.177 |
N | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | 7,723 | |||||
Residual DoF | 750 | 736 | 734 | 734 | 708 | |||||
Notes: All estimates are weighted and inference accounts for the complex survey design. | ||||||||||
1 *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001 | ||||||||||
2 SE = Standard Error |
7 Appendix: Multiple Imputation
Characteristic | Beta1 | SE2 |
---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 3.3*** | 0.130 |
Gender | ||
Male | — | — |
Female | -0.47*** | 0.051 |
Non-Binary+ | -1.2*** | 0.178 |
Ethnicity | ||
White | — | — |
Mixed | -0.09 | 0.115 |
Black | 0.15* | 0.071 |
Asian | 0.18* | 0.079 |
Other | 0.18 | 0.174 |
SES Quintile Group | ||
Q1 (Low) | — | — |
Q2 | -0.10 | 0.080 |
Q3 | 0.04 | 0.081 |
Q4 | -0.01 | 0.076 |
Q5 (High) | 0.07 | 0.081 |
Social Provisions Scale | 0.64*** | 0.021 |
Adverse Event Index | -0.50*** | 0.037 |
Wave 1 Survey Month | ||
Sep 2021 | — | — |
Oct 2021 | 0.03 | 0.057 |
Nov 2021 | 0.37* | 0.154 |
Dec 2021 | 0.09 | 0.108 |
Jan 2022 | 0.33 | 0.244 |
Feb 2022 | -0.43* | 0.212 |
Mar 2022 | -0.12 | 0.081 |
Apr 2022 | -0.09 | 0.088 |
N | 9,307 | |
Notes: All estimates are weighted and inference accounts for the complex survey design. Minimum residual degrees of freedom in any of the 10 imputations = 748 | ||
1 *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001 | ||
2 SE = Standard Error |
Characteristic | Beta1 | SE2 |
---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 4.8*** | 0.130 |
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing | ||
No | — | — |
Yes | -1.0*** | 0.055 |
Gender | ||
Male | — | — |
Female | -0.32*** | 0.049 |
Non-Binary+ | -0.97*** | 0.166 |
Ethnicity | ||
White | — | — |
Mixed | -0.16 | 0.109 |
Black | 0.09 | 0.066 |
Asian | 0.06 | 0.091 |
Other | 0.30 | 0.162 |
SES Quintile Group | ||
Q1 (Low) | — | — |
Q2 | 0.09 | 0.078 |
Q3 | 0.08 | 0.075 |
Q4 | 0.15 | 0.077 |
Q5 (High) | 0.26** | 0.087 |
Social Provisions Scale | 0.39*** | 0.021 |
Adverse Event Index | -0.39*** | 0.035 |
Wave 2 Survey Month | ||
October 2022 | — | — |
November 2022 | -0.11* | 0.052 |
December 2022 | 0.09 | 0.104 |
January 2023 | 0.25 | 0.289 |
February 2023 | 0.34 | 0.208 |
March 2023 | 0.41* | 0.182 |
April 2023 | 0.01 | 0.178 |
N | 9,307 | |
Notes: All estimates are weighted and inference accounts for the complex survey design. Minimum residual degrees of freedom in any of the 10 imputations = 748 | ||
1 *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001 | ||
2 SE = Standard Error |
Characteristic | Beta1 | SE2 |
---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 3.3*** | 0.134 |
Wave 1 Wellbeing | 0.41*** | 0.016 |
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing | ||
No | — | — |
Yes | -0.75*** | 0.051 |
Gender | ||
Male | — | — |
Female | -0.16*** | 0.043 |
Non-Binary+ | -0.56*** | 0.162 |
Ethnicity | ||
White | — | — |
Mixed | -0.11 | 0.089 |
Black | 0.03 | 0.057 |
Asian | -0.03 | 0.083 |
Other | 0.24 | 0.149 |
SES Quintile Group | ||
Q1 (Low) | — | — |
Q2 | 0.13 | 0.071 |
Q3 | 0.05 | 0.067 |
Q4 | 0.13 | 0.070 |
Q5 (High) | 0.17* | 0.073 |
Social Provisions Scale | 0.13*** | 0.021 |
Adverse Event Index | -0.22*** | 0.031 |
Wave 1 Survey Month | ||
Sep 2021 | — | — |
Oct 2021 | 0.01 | 0.052 |
Nov 2021 | 0.24* | 0.122 |
Dec 2021 | 0.36** | 0.113 |
Jan 2022 | 0.19 | 0.175 |
Feb 2022 | 0.23 | 0.285 |
Mar 2022 | -0.03 | 0.069 |
Apr 2022 | -0.03 | 0.075 |
Wave 2 Survey Month | ||
October 2022 | — | — |
November 2022 | -0.08 | 0.047 |
December 2022 | 0.13 | 0.095 |
January 2023 | 0.40 | 0.264 |
February 2023 | 0.20 | 0.201 |
March 2023 | 0.44** | 0.151 |
April 2023 | 0.00 | 0.176 |
N | 9,307 | |
Notes: All estimates are weighted and inference accounts for the complex survey design. Minimum residual degrees of freedom in any of the 10 imputations = 740 | ||
1 *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001 | ||
2 SE = Standard Error |
Characteristic | Beta1 | SE2 |
---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 3.5*** | 0.167 |
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing | ||
No | — | — |
Yes | -0.84*** | 0.051 |
Gender | ||
Male | — | — |
Female | -0.45*** | 0.050 |
Non-Binary+ | -1.1*** | 0.184 |
Ethnicity | ||
White | — | — |
Mixed | -0.11 | 0.114 |
Black | 0.15* | 0.072 |
Asian | 0.19* | 0.080 |
Other | 0.19 | 0.170 |
Parental Education | -0.02 | 0.045 |
Housing Tenure | -0.05 | 0.057 |
IDACI Quintile Group | ||
1 (High Deprivation) | — | — |
2 | 0.07 | 0.077 |
3 | 0.03 | 0.084 |
4 | 0.17* | 0.082 |
5 (Low Deprivation) | 0.19* | 0.087 |
Social Provisions Scale | 0.67*** | 0.020 |
Wave 1 Survey Month | ||
Sep 2021 | — | — |
Oct 2021 | 0.02 | 0.056 |
Nov 2021 | 0.33* | 0.158 |
Dec 2021 | 0.08 | 0.110 |
Jan 2022 | 0.36 | 0.246 |
Feb 2022 | -0.53** | 0.202 |
Mar 2022 | -0.12 | 0.080 |
Apr 2022 | -0.07 | 0.088 |
N | 9,307 | |
Notes: All estimates are weighted and inference accounts for the complex survey design. Minimum residual degrees of freedom in any of the 10 imputations = 746 | ||
1 *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001 | ||
2 SE = Standard Error |
Characteristic | Beta1 | SE2 |
---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 4.9*** | 0.170 |
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing | ||
No | — | — |
Yes | -1.2*** | 0.054 |
Gender | ||
Male | — | — |
Female | -0.38*** | 0.050 |
Non-Binary+ | -1.1*** | 0.168 |
Ethnicity | ||
White | — | — |
Mixed | -0.16 | 0.110 |
Black | 0.08 | 0.067 |
Asian | 0.11 | 0.095 |
Other | 0.32 | 0.165 |
Parental Education | 0.03 | 0.044 |
Housing Tenure | -0.17** | 0.056 |
IDACI Quintile Group | ||
1 (High Deprivation) | — | — |
2 | 0.15* | 0.077 |
3 | 0.13 | 0.081 |
4 | 0.19* | 0.081 |
5 (Low Deprivation) | 0.20* | 0.086 |
Social Provisions Scale | 0.43*** | 0.021 |
Wave 2 Survey Month | ||
October 2022 | — | — |
November 2022 | -0.10* | 0.052 |
December 2022 | 0.08 | 0.108 |
January 2023 | 0.23 | 0.280 |
February 2023 | 0.34 | 0.205 |
March 2023 | 0.40* | 0.181 |
April 2023 | 0.00 | 0.174 |
N | 9,307 | |
Notes: All estimates are weighted and inference accounts for the complex survey design. Minimum residual degrees of freedom in any of the 10 imputations = 747 | ||
1 *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001 | ||
2 SE = Standard Error |
Characteristic | Beta1 | SE2 |
---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 3.3*** | 0.171 |
Wave 1 Wellbeing | 0.43*** | 0.016 |
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing | ||
No | — | — |
Yes | -0.81*** | 0.051 |
Gender | ||
Male | — | — |
Female | -0.19*** | 0.043 |
Non-Binary+ | -0.61*** | 0.162 |
Ethnicity | ||
White | — | — |
Mixed | -0.10 | 0.089 |
Black | 0.01 | 0.057 |
Asian | 0.01 | 0.086 |
Other | 0.25 | 0.151 |
Parental Education | 0.05 | 0.037 |
Housing Tenure | -0.15** | 0.050 |
IDACI Quintile Group | ||
1 (High Deprivation) | — | — |
2 | 0.12 | 0.067 |
3 | 0.12 | 0.075 |
4 | 0.10 | 0.072 |
5 (Low Deprivation) | 0.10 | 0.074 |
Social Provisions Scale | 0.14*** | 0.021 |
Wave 1 Survey Month | ||
Sep 2021 | — | — |
Oct 2021 | 0.01 | 0.052 |
Nov 2021 | 0.24* | 0.120 |
Dec 2021 | 0.36** | 0.114 |
Jan 2022 | 0.21 | 0.172 |
Feb 2022 | 0.23 | 0.285 |
Mar 2022 | -0.04 | 0.070 |
Apr 2022 | -0.02 | 0.076 |
Wave 2 Survey Month | ||
October 2022 | — | — |
November 2022 | -0.08 | 0.047 |
December 2022 | 0.12 | 0.096 |
January 2023 | 0.40 | 0.256 |
February 2023 | 0.18 | 0.197 |
March 2023 | 0.44** | 0.150 |
April 2023 | -0.01 | 0.172 |
N | 9,307 | |
Notes: All estimates are weighted and inference accounts for the complex survey design. Minimum residual degrees of freedom in any of the 10 imputations = 739 | ||
1 *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001 | ||
2 SE = Standard Error |
Characteristic | Beta1 | SE2 |
---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 4.0*** | 0.181 |
Adverse Event Tercile Groups | ||
Low | — | — |
Medium | -0.34*** | 0.062 |
High | -0.76*** | 0.071 |
Negative continuing impact of pandemic on mental wellbeing | ||
No | — | — |
Yes | -0.70*** | 0.052 |
Gender | ||
Male | — | — |
Female | -0.39*** | 0.050 |
Non-Binary+ | -1.0*** | 0.178 |
Ethnicity | ||
White | — | — |
Mixed | -0.10 | 0.115 |
Black | 0.12 | 0.071 |
Asian | 0.15 | 0.078 |
Other | 0.18 | 0.171 |
Parental Education | -0.03 | 0.044 |
Housing Tenure | -0.02 | 0.056 |
IDACI Quintile Group | ||
1 (High Deprivation) | — | — |
2 | 0.04 | 0.076 |
3 | 0.01 | 0.084 |
4 | 0.17* | 0.081 |
5 (Low Deprivation) | 0.15 | 0.086 |
Social Provisions Scale | 0.63*** | 0.021 |
Wave 1 Survey Month | ||
Sep 2021 |